[1] Lashley K S. Brain mechanisms and intelligence: a quantitative study of injuries to the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 1929 [2] Scoville W B, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiat, 1957, 20: 11~21 [3] Squire L R, Knowlton B, Muser G. The structure and organization of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 1993, 44: 453~495 [4] Warrington E K, Weiskrantz L. New method of testing long-term retention with special reference to amnesia patients. Nature(London), 1968, 217: 972~974 [5] Heindel W C, Salmon D P, Shults C W, et al. Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: A comparison of Alzheimer′s, Huntington′s and Parkinson′s disease patients. Journal of Neuroscience, 1989, 9: 582~587 [6] Keane M M, Gabrieli J D, Fennema A C, et al. Evidence for a dissociation between perceptual and conceptual priming in Alzheimer′s disease. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1991, 105: 326~342 [7] Keane M M, Gabrieli J D, Mapstone H C, et al. Double dissociation of memory capacities after bilateral occipital-lobe or medial temporal-lobe lesions. Brain, 1995, 118: 1129~1148 [8] Monti L A, Gabrieli J D, Reminger S L, et al. Differential effects of aging and Alzheimer′s disease upon conceptual implicit and explicit memory. Neurology, 1996, 10: 101~112 [9] Keane M M, Gabrieli J D, Noland J S, et al. Normal perceptual priming of orthographically illegal words in amnesia. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 1995, 1: 425~433 [10] Vaidya C J, Gabrieli J D, Momti L A, et al. Dissociation between two forms of conceptual priming in Alzheimer′s disease. Neuropsychology, 1999, 13: 516~524 [11] Fleischman D A, Vaidya C J, Lang K L, et al. A dissociation between perceptual explicit and implicit memory processes. Memory and Cognition, 1997, 35: 42~57 [12] Squire L R, Ojemann J G, Miezin F M, et al. Activation of hippocampus in normal humans: A functional anatomical study of memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 1992, 89: 1837~1841 [13] Buckner R L, Petersen S E, Ojemann J G, et al. Functional anatomical studies of explicit and implicit memory retrieval tasks. Journal of Neuroscience, 1995, 15: 12~29 [14] Buckner R L, Koutstal W, Schacter D L, et al. Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal componenta of priming. Brain, 2000, 123: 620~640 [15] Peterson S E, Fox P T, Snyder A Z, et al. Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-linked stimuli. Science, 1990, 249: 1041~1044 [16] Schacter D L, Badgaiyan R D, Alpert N M. Visual and word-stem completion priming within and across modalities: A PET study. Neuroreport, 1998, 10: 2061~2065 [17] Yasuno F, Nishikawa T, Tokunaga H, et al. The neural basis of perceptual and conceptual word priming: a PET study. Cortex, 2000, 36: 59~69 [18] Buckner R L, Koutstal W. Functional neuroimaging studies of encoding, priming, and explicit memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 1995, 95: 891~898 [19] Schacter D L, Buckner R L. Priming and the brain: Review. Neuron, 1998, 20: 185~195 [20] Raichle M E, Fiez J A, Videen T O, et al. Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during non-motor learning. Cerebral Cortex, 1994, 4: 8~26 [21] Badgaiyan R D, Schacter D L, Alpert N M. Auditory priming within and across modalities: evidence from positron emission tomography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1999, 11: 337~348 [22] Carlesimo G A, Fadda L, Sabbadini M, et al. Visual repetition priming for words relies on access to the visual input lexicon: evidence from a dyslexic patient. Neuropsychologia, 1994, 32: 1089~1100 [23] Gabrieli J D, Fleischman D A, Keane M M, et al. Double dissociation between memory systems underlying explicit and implicit memory in the human brain. Psychological Science, 1995, 6: 76~82 [24] Srinivas K, Breedin S D, Coslett B, et al. Intact perceptual priming in a patient with damage to the anterior inferior temporal lobes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1997, 9: 490~511 [25] Geva A, Moscovitch M, Leach L. Perceptual priming of proper names in young and older normal adults and a patient with prosopanomia. Neuropsychology, 1997, 11: 232~242 [26] Elizabeth A K, Simona S, Stefano F C, et al. Role of the anterior temporal lobe in repetition and semantic priming: Evidence from a patient with a category-specific deficit. Neuropsychologia, 2003, 41: 71~84 [27] Buckner R L, Wheeler M E, Maccotta L, et al. Code-specific repetition priming in frontal and posterior cortex. Neuroimage, 2001, 13(6): 642 [28] Yang J, Weng X, Guan L, et al. Involvement of the medial temporal lobe in priming for new associations. Neuropsychologia, 2003, 41: 818~829 [29] 王常生, 于生元, 李恩中. 基底神经节损伤对知觉性启动效应影响的进一步研究. 心理学报, 2000, 32(3): 306~310 [30] 杨炯炯, 翁旭初, 管林初等. 额叶在启动效应中的作用机制: 对额叶损伤病人颜色命名和词命名任务的分析. 心理学报, 2003, 35(5): 610~616 [31] Price C J, Mummery C J, Moore C J, et al. Delineating necessary and sufficient neural systems with functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1999, 11: 371~382 [32] Gruppuso V, Lindsay D S, Kelley C M. The process-dissociation procedure and similarity: Defining and estimating recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1997, 23(2): 259~278 |