[1] Posner M I. Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1980, 32: 3~25 [2] Wright R D, Ward L M. Shifts of visual attention: A historical and methodological overview. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1994, 48(2): 151~166 [3] Kanwisher N, Wojciulik E. Visual attention: insights from brain imaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2000, 1: 91~100 [4] Gawryszewski L D, Riggio L, Rizzolatti G. Movements of attention in three spatial dimensions and the meaning of "neutral" cues. Neuropsychologica, 1987, 25: 19~29 [5] Andersen G J, Kramer A F. The limits of focused attention in three-dimensional space. Perception & Psychophysics, 1993, 53: 658~667 [6] He Z J, Nakayama K. Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1995, 92: 11155~11159 [7] Robertson L C, Kim M. Effects of perceived space on spatial attention. Psychological Science, 1999, 10(1): 76~79 [8] Previc F. Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in human: its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 1990, 13: 519~575. [9] He S, Cavanagh P, Intriligator J. Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness. Nature, 1996, 383: 334~337 [10] Rubin N, Nakayama K, Shapely R. Enhanced perception of illusory contours in the lower versus upper visual hemifields. Science, 1996, 271: 651~653 [11] Zhou W, King W. Attentional sensitivity and asymmetries of vertical saccade generation in monkey. Vision Research, 2002, 42: 771~779 [12] Atchley P, Kramer A, Anderson G. Spatial cueing in a stereoscopic display: evidence for a depth-aware attentional focus. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1997, 4: 524~529 |