北京语言学院语言教学研究所(编). (1986). 现代汉语频率词典. 北京: 北京语言学院出版社. 毕鸿燕, 胡伟, 翁旭初. (2006). 汉语形声字声旁家族大小对整字发音的影响. 心理学报, 38(6), 791-797 陈新葵, 张积家. (2012). 义符熟悉性对低频形声字词汇通达的影响. 心理学报, 44(7), 882-895 陈原 (编). (1993). 现代汉语用字信息分析. 上海: 上海教育出版社. 韩布新. (1998). 汉字识别中部件的频率效应. 心理科学, 21(3), 193-195 李燕, 康加深. (1993). 现代汉语形声字声符研究. 见 陈原(编). 现代汉语用字信息分析(pp. 68-98). 上海: 上海教育出版社. 谢敏, 杨青青, 王权红. (2016). 词汇加工研究中的P200成分. 心理学进展, 6(2), 114-120 Barnea, A., & Breznitz, Z. (1998). Phonological and orthographic processing of Hebrew words: Electrophysiological aspects. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 159(4), 492-504, doi: 10.1080/00221329809596166. Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M. H., Echallier, J. F., & Pernier, J. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: Time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(3), 235-260, doi: 10.1162/089892999563373. Carreiras, M., Gillon-Dowens, M., Vergara, M., & Perea, M. (2009). Are vowels and consonants processed differently? Event-related potential evidence with a delayed letter paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(2), 275-288, doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21023. Dien, J. (2009). The neurocognitive basis of reading single words as seen through early latency ERPs: A model of converging pathways. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 10-22, doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.04.013. Ding, G. S., Peng, D. L., & Taft, M. (2004). The nature of the mental representation of radicals in Chinese: A priming study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 530-539, doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.530. Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1993). Masked partial-word priming in visual word recognition: Effects of positional letter frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(4), 951-964. Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2006). On the time course of visual word recognition: An event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. Journal of Cognitive and Neuroscience, 18(10), 1631-1643, doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1631. Hsu, C. H., Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2009). Orthographic combinability and phonological consistency effects in reading Chinese phonograms: An event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 108(1), 56-66, doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.002. Kong, L. Y., Zhang, B., Zhang, J. X., & Kang, C. P. (2012). P200 can be modulated by orthography alone in reading Chinese words. Neuroscience Letters, 529(2), 161-165, doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.09.028. Kong, L. Y., Zhang, J. X., Kang, C. P., Du, Y. C., Zhang, B., & Wang, S. P. (2010). P200 and phonological processing in Chinese word recognition. Neuroscience Letters, 473(1), 37-41, doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.02.014. Kramer, A. F., & Donchin, E. (1987). Brain potentials as indices of orthographic and phonological interaction during word matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13(1), 76-86, doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.1.76. Kutas, M., & van Petten, C. (1994). Psycholinguistics electrified: Event-related brain potential investigations. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 83-143). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Chan, W. H., Hsu, C. H., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. (2007). Temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: An event-related potentials study. Neuroreport, 18(2), 147-151, doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328010d4e4. Liu, Y., Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2003). ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1231-1247, doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.6.1231. Misra, M., & Holcomb, P. J. (2003). Event-related potential indices of masked repetition priming. Psychophysiology, 40(1), 115-130, doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00012. Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (1998). Effects of syllable frequency and syllable neighborhood frequency in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(1), 134-144, doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.1.134. Rugg, M. D. (1987). Dissociation of semantic priming, word and non-word repetition effects by event-related potentials. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Sections A, 39(1), 123-148, doi: 10.1080/02724988743000060. Savill, N. J., & Thierry, G. (2012). Decoding ability makes waves in reading: Deficient interactions between attention and phonological analysis in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1553-1564, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.008. Wu, Y., Mo, D. Y., Tsang, Y. K., & Chen, H. C. (2012). ERPs reveal sub-lexical processing in Chinese character recognition. Neuroscience Letters, 514(2), 164-168, doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.02.080. Yum, Y. N., Law, S. P., Su, I. F., Lau, K. Y. D., & Mo, K. N. (2014). An ERP study of effects of regularity and consistency in delayed naming and lexicality judgment in a logographic writing system. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 315. Zhang, Q., Zhang, J. X., & Kong, L. Y. (2009). An ERP study on the time course of phonological and semantic activation in Chinese word recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73(3), 235-245, doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.04.001. Zhou, L., Fong, M. C. M., Minett, J. W., Peng, G., & Wang, W. S. Y. (2014). Pre-lexical phonological processing in reading Chinese characters: An ERP study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 30, 14-26, doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.03.002. Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1245-1265, doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1245. Ziegler, J. C., Benraïss, A., & Besson, M. (1999). From print to meaning: An electrophysiological investigation of the role of phonology in accessing word meaning. Psychophysiology, 36(6), 775-785, doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3660775. |