[1] Joormann J. Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion, 2004, 18 (1): 125-147 [2] May C P, Kane M J, Hasher L. Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 1995, 118: 35-54 [3] Wagner M, Baving L, Berg P, et al. An ERP investigation of semantic priming, repetition priming, and negative priming in schi-zophrenic patients. Journal of Psychophysiology, 2006, 20: 195-211 [4] Troche S J, Trenkwalder C, Morelli-Canelo M, et al. Unimpaired negative but enhanced positive in Parkinson′s disease: Evidence from an identity and a location priming task. Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44: 1811-1821 [5] Connelly S L, Hasher L. Aging and inhibition of spatial location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005, 19: 1238-1250 [6] Mayr S, Buchner A. Negative priming as a memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years of negative priming research. Journal of Psychology, 2007, 215: 35-51 [7] Pritchard V E, Neumann E. Negative Priming Effects in Children engaged in Nonspatial Tasks: Evidence for Early Development of an Intact Inhibitory Mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 2004, 40: 191-203 [8] Moritz S, Ruff C, Wilke U, Andresen B, Krausz M, Naber D. Negative priming in schizophrenia: Effects of masking and prime presentation time. Schizophrenia Research, 2001, 48: 291-299 [9] Buchner A, Zabal A, Mayr S. Auditory, visual, and cross-modal negative priming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2003, 10(4): 917-923 [10] Chao H F, Yeh Y Y. Location negative priming in identity discrimination relies on location repetition. Perception and Psychophysics, 2005, 67(5): 789-801 [11] Leboe J P, Mondor T A, Leboe L C. Feature mismatch effects in auditory negative priming: Interference as dependent on salient aspects of prior episodes. Perception and Psychophysics, 2006, 68(6): 897-910 [12] Tipper S P. Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 2001, 54A: 321-343 [13] Frings C, Groh-Bordin C. Electrophysiological correlates of visual identity negative priming. Brain Reasearch, 2007, 1177: 82-93 [14] Hinojosa J A, Pozo M A, Méndez-Bértolo C, et al. Event-related potential correlates of visual identity negative priming unbiased by trial-by-trial effects. Brain and Cognition, 2009, 69: 531-537 [15] Hinojosa J A, Villarino A, Pozo M A, et al. An event-related potentials study of identity positive and negative priming. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 2007, 66: 48-55 [16] Kathmann N, Bogdahn B, Endrass T. Event-related brain potential variations during location and identity negative priming. Neuroscience Letters, 2006, 394: 53-56 [17] Ceballos N A, Nixon S J, Tivis R. Substance abuse-related P300 differences in response to an implicit memory task. Progress in Neuro Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 2003, 27: 157-164 [18] Mayr S, Niedeggen M, Buchner A, Pietrowsky R. ERP correlates of auditory negative priming. Cognition, 2003, 90: 11-21 [19] Mayr S, Niedeggen M, Buchner A, Orgs G. The level of reaction time determines the ERP correlates of auditory negative priming. Journal of Psychophysiology, 2006, 20: 186-194 [20] Heil M, Rolke B. Unattended Distractor-lnduced Priming in a Visual Selective Attention Task: N400 Effects in the Absence of RT Effects. Journal of Psychophysiology, 2004, 18: 164-169 [21] Tobias E, Joy H. Where Memory Meets Attention:Neural Substrates of Negative Priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2005, 17: 1774-1784 [22] Gibbons H. An event-related potential investigation of varieties of negative priming. Journal of Psychophysiology, 2006, 20: 170-185 [23] Gibbons H, Rammsayer T H, Stahl J. Multiple sources of positive-and negative-priming effects: An event-related potential study. Memory and Cognition, 2006, 34, 172-186 [24] Ruge H, Naumann E. Brain-electrical correlates of negative location priming under sustained and transient attentional context conditions. Journal of Psychophysiology, 2006, 20: 160-169 [25] Wright C I, McMullin K, et a1. Brain correlates of negative visual spatial priming in healthy children. Psychiatry Research, 2005, 139: 41-52 [26] Wright C I, Keuthen N J, et a1. Brain correlates of negative and positive visuospatial priming in adults. Neuroimage, 2006, 30: 83-91 [27] Kugeger F, Fischer R, Heinecke A, et al. An fMRI investigation into the neural mechanisms of spatial attentional selection in a location-based negative priming task. Brain Research, 2007, 1174: 110-119 [28] 陈文峰, 焦书兰. 选择性注意中的客体与空间因素. 心理科学, 2005, 28(2): 395-397 [29] Kim M-S, Cave R C. Perceptual grouping via spatial selection in a focused-attention task. Vision Research, 2001, 41: 611-624 [30] Vecera S P. Dissociating‘what’and‘how’in visual form agnosia: a computational investigation. Neuropsychologia, 2002, 40: 187-204 [31] 魏景汉, 罗跃嘉. 认知时间相关脑电位教程. 北京: 经济日报出版社, 2002 [32] Lavie N, Hirst A, Forkert J W, et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2004, 133(3): 339-354 [33] Vogel E K, McCollough A W, Machizawa M G. Neural Measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 2005, 438: 500-503 [34] Fockert J W, Rees G, Frith C D, et al. The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 2001, 291(5509): 1803-1806 [35] Do-Joon Yi, Woodman G F, Widders D, et al. Neural fate of ignored stimuli: dissociable effects of perceptual and working memory load. Nature Neuroscience, 2004, 7(9): 992-996 [36] 罗良, 刘兆敏, 林崇德. 人类视空间工作记忆分离的证据与机制. 心理科学进展, 2007, 15(3): 394-400 [37] 严霄霏, 苏彦捷, 王甦. 儿童整体—局部加工中的负启动现象. 心理科学, 2006, 29(3): 588-592 [38] 沈德立, 王敬欣. 分心抑制与年龄关系的位置负启动效应实验研究. 心理与行为研究, 2003, 1(1): 19-22 [39] 张丽华, 白学军, 张秀红. 创造性思维能力高、低大学生特性负启动效应的实验研究. 心理与行为研究, 2007, 5(2): 112-115 |