刘超, 买晓琴, 傅小兰. (2004). 不同注意条件下的空间—数字反应编码联合反应. 心理学报, 36(6), 671-680. 刘超, 买晓琴, 傅小兰.(2005). 内源性注意与外源性注意条件对数字加工的不同影响.心理学报, 37(2), 167-177. 潘运, 白学军, 沈德立. (2010). 内源性注意和外源性注意条件下SOA变化对数字距离效应的影响. 心理科学, 33(3), 521-524. 潘运, 白学军, 沈德立. (2011). 内源性注意和外源性注意条件下线索提示有效性对数字距离效应的影响. 西南大学学报(自然科学版), 33(8), 167-172. 隋光远, 吴燕, 曹晓华. (2006). 数困儿童在内源和外源注意条件下数字比较的眼动研究. 心理科学, 29(3), 583-587. Ansari, D., Lyons, I. M., van Eimeren, L., & Xu, F. (2007). Linking visual attention and number processing in the brain: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in small and large symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1845–1853. Berger, A., Henik, A., & Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 207–221. Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102(1), 101–117. Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. New York: Oxford University Press. Dehaene, S., & Akhanvein, R. (1995). Attention, automaticity and the levels of representation in number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 21(2), 313–326. Doallo, S., Lorenzo-López, L., Vizoso, C., Rodríguez Holguín, S., Amenedo, E., Bará, S., et al. (2005). Modulations of the visual N1 component of event-related potentials by central and peripheral cueing. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116(4), 807–820. Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 66(6), 555–556. Holloway, D. I., & Ansari, D. (2008). Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children’s development of number comparison. Developmental Science, 11(5), 644–649. Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215(5109), 1519–1520. Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J., & Miller, E. K. (2002). Representations of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297, 1708–1711. Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intrapa-rietal sulcus. Neuron, 44, 547–555. Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Riviere, D., & LeBihan, D. (2001). Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. Neuroimage, 14(5), 1013–1026. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(2), 3–25. Rubinsten, O., Henik ,A., Berger, A., & Shahar-Shalev, S. (2002). The development of internal representation of magnitude and their association with Arabic numerals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81(1), 74–92. Sz?cs, D., Soltész, F., Jármi, é., et al. (2007). The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study. Behavioral and Brain Function, 30(1), 3–23. van Opstal. F., & Verguts, T. (2011). The origins of the numerical distance effect: The same-different task.Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(1), 112–120. |