戴庆夏. (2010). 藏缅语族语言研究(五). 昆明:云南民族出版社. 戴庆厦, 刘菊黄, 傅爱兰. (1989). 关于我国藏缅语族系属分类问题. 云南民族大学学报:哲学社会科学版, (3), 82-92 戴庆厦, 朱艳华. (2010). 藏缅语选择疑问范畴句法结构的演变链. 汉语学报, (2), 2-13 戴庆厦, 朱艳华. (2011). 20年来汉藏语系的语言类型学研究. 云南民族大学学报:哲学社会科学版, 28(5), 131-137 郭卜乐. (2005). 马斯洛的需要层次理论. 教师博览, (9), 38-39 金尚会. (2005). 中国彝族文化的民族学研究 (博士学位论文). 中央民族大学. 井祥贵. (2011). 纳西族学校民族文化传承机制研究 (博士学位论文). 西南大学. 刘正发(彝名:阿里瓦萨). (2007). 凉山彝族家支文化传承的教育人类学研究——以云南省宁蒗彝族自治县金古忍石家支为个案 (博士学位论文).中央民族大学. 王娟, 张积家, 谢书书, 袁爱玲. (2011). 结合东巴文学习汉字对幼儿汉字字形记忆的影响. 心理学报, 43(5), 519-533 王锋. (2005). 白族语言文字研究的重要课题及展望. 大理文化, (5), 58-61 杨丽珠, 董光恒. (2005). 3~5岁幼儿自我控制能力结构研究. 心理发展与教育, 21(4), 7-12 杨丽珠, 王江洋. (2007). 儿童4岁时自我延迟满足能力对其9岁时学校社会交往能力预期的追踪. 心理学报, 39(4), 668-678 杨丽珠, 王江洋, 刘文, Cuskelly, M., & Zhang, A. R (2005). 3~5岁幼儿自我延迟满足的发展特点及其中澳跨文化比较. 心理学报, 37(2), 224-232 杨丽珠, 徐丽敏, 王江洋. (2003). 四种注意情境下幼儿自我延迟满足的实验研究. 心理发展与教育, 19(4), 1-6 余海波. (2008). 纳西族中学生的民族认同意识. 中央民族大学学报:哲学社会科学版, 35(4), 55-59 赵金元, 饶清翠, 凡丽. (2009). 白族文化的包容性及其现实意义. 中国发展, 9(3), 80-85 朱志强. (1989). 马斯洛的需要层次理论述评. 武汉大学学报(人文科学版), (2), 124-126 Beck, L., Kumschick, I. R., Eid, M., & Klann-Delius, G. (2012). Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. Emotion, 12(3), 503-514. Bembenutty, H. (2009). Academic delay of gratification, self-regulation of learning, gender differences, and expectancy-value. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(3), 347-352. Beran, M. J., & Evans, T. A. (2012). Language-trained chimpanzees (pan troglodytes) delay gratification by choosing token exchange over immediate reward consumption. American Journal of Primatology, 74(9), 864-870. Cuskelly, M. M., & Stubbins, P. (2007). Self-imposed delay of gratification in adolescents with down syndrome. Journal on Developmental Disabilities, 12(1-Supplement 2), 19-28. Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(3), 259-268. Forstmeier, S., Drobetz, R., & Maercker, A. (2011). The delay of gratification test for adults:validating a behavioral measure of self-motivation in a sample of older people. Motivation & Emotion, 35(2), 118-134. Funder, D. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1983). Delay of gratification:Some longitudinal personality correlates. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 44(6), 1198-1213. Gaddes, W. H., & Crockett, D. J. (1975). The spreen-benton aphasia tests, normative data as a measure of normal language development. Brain & Language, 2, 257-280. Greenfield, P. M., & Cocking, R. R. (2014). Cross-cultural roots of minority child development. New York:Psychology Press. Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2011). The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards:a life history theory approach. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 100(6), 1015-1026. Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science, 344(6186), 862-867. Imada, T., Carlson, S. M., & Itakura, S. (2013). East-West cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Developmental Science, 16(2), 198-208. Ji, L.-J., Zhang, Z. Y., & Nisbett, R. E. (2004). Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 57-65. Klein, D., Milner, B., Zatorre, R. J., Meyer, E., & Evans, A. C. (1995). The neural substrates underlying word generation:A bilingual functional-imaging study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 92(7), 2899-2903. Kuwabara, M. (2011). Early origin of cross-cultural differences in attention to objects (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Indiana University. Liu, L., Feng, T. Y., Suo, T., Lee, K., & Li, H. (2012). Adapting to the destitute situations:Poverty cues lead to Short-Term choice. PLoS One, 7(4), e33950. Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind:Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622-646. Moffitt, Terrie E, Arseneault, Louise, Belsky, Daniel, Dickson, Nigel, Hancox, Robert J, Harrington, HonaLee,.. Ross, Stephen. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693-2698. Mischel, W. (1961). Delay of gratification, need for achievement, and acquiescence in another culture. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(3), 543-552. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 687-696. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244(4907), 933-938. Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26(5), 653-684. Olson, S. L., & Hoza, B. (1993). Preschool development antecedents of conduct problems in children beginning school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22(1), 60-67. Shukla, S. (2015). Relative or absolute? the impact of reward size on 3-and 4-year old children's ability to delay gratification. University of Otago. Zhang, L. L., Karabenick, S. A., Maruno, S. I., & Lauermann, F. (2011). Academic delay of gratification and children's study time allocation as a function of proximity to consequential academic goals. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 77-94. |