北京语言学院语言教学研究所. (1986). 现代汉语频率词典. 北京:北京语言学院出版社. 蔡治. (2003). 视角对汉字识别影响的研究 (硕士学位论文). 西南师范大学, 重庆. 常玉林, 王丹烁, 周蔚. (2016). 字形判断过程中的整体与局部优先效应:来自反应时和眼动指标的证据. 心理科学, 39(5), 1040-1044 陈宝国, 王立新, 彭聃龄. (2003). 汉字识别中形音义激活时间进程的研究(II). 心理学报, 35(5), 576-581 罗春荣, 纪桂萍, 房路江. (1987). 从汉字识别看视觉信息检测的整体优先性. 心理学报, 19(2), 184-189 罗艳琳, 王鹏, 李秀军, 石雅琪, 陈墨, 王培培, … 罗跃嘉. (2010). 汉字认知过程中整字对部件的影响. 心理学报, 42(6), 683-694 张积家. (2007). 整体与部分的意义关系对汉字知觉的影响. 心理科学, 30(5), 1095-1098, doi:10.3969/j.issn.1671-6981.2007.05.017 张积家, 王惠萍. (2001). 声旁与整字的音段、声调关系对形声字命名的影响. 心理学报, 33(3), 2-6 张积家, 王娟, 陈新葵. (2014). 义符研究20年:理论探讨、实验证据和加工模型. 心理科学进展, 22(3), 381-399 赵欣, 袁杰, 徐依宁, 傅世敏. (2014). 基于物体的注意的机制与影响因素. 心理科学进展, 22(11), 1708-1722 Chen, Y. C., & Yeh, S. L. (2015). Binding radicals in Chinese character recognition:Evidence from repetition blindness. Journal of Memory and Language, 78, 47-63, doi:10.1016/j.jml.2014.10.002. Chen, Y. C., & Yeh, S. L. (2017). Examining radical position and function in Chinese character recognition using the repetition blindness paradigm. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 32(1), 37-54, doi:10.1080/23273798.2016.1227856. Kinchla, R. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing:"Top-down", "bottom-up" or "middle-out". Perception & Psychophysics, 25(3), 225-231. Li, Y., & Kang, J. S. (1993). Analysis of phonetics of the ideophonetic characters in modern Chinese. In Y. Chen (Ed.), Information analysis of usage of characters in modern Chinese (pp. 84-98). Shanghai:Shanghai Education Publisher. Li, X. S., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words:Beyond Gestalt principles. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(5), 945-949. Liu, D. L., Wang, Y. H., & Zhou, X. L. (2011). Lexical-and perceptual-based object effects in the two-rectangle cueing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 138(3), 397-404, doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.09.008. Liu, T. Y., Chuk, T. Y., Yeh, S. L., & Hsiao, J. H. (2016). Transfer of perceptual expertise:The case of simplified and Traditional Chinese character recognition. Cognitive Science, 40(8), 1941-1968, doi:10.1111/cogs.12307. Luo, C. M., Proctor, R. W., Weng, X. C., & Li, X. S. (2014). Spatial Stroop interference occurs in the processing of radicals of ideogrammic compounds. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 715-720. Malcolm, G., & Shomstein, S. (2013). High-level semantic information affects attentional allocation within and between objects. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 770, doi:10.1167/13.9.770. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees:The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383, doi:10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3. Taft, M. (2006). Processing of characters by native Chinese readers. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates, & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), The handbook of east Asian psycholinguistics, Vol. 1:Chinese (pp. 237-249). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press. Tao, R. V., Au, T. K., & Hsiao, J. H. (2014). Perceptual expertise:Can sensorimotor experience change holistic processing and left-side bias? Psychological Science, 25(9), 1757-1767, doi:10.1177/0956797614541284. Yeh, S. L., Chou, W. L., & Ho, P. (2017). Lexical processing of Chinese sub-character components:Semantic activation of phonetic radicals as revealed by the Stroop effect. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 15782, doi:10.1038/s41598-017-15536-w. Yuan, J., & Fu, S. M. (2014). Attention can operate on semantic objects defined by individual Chinese characters. Visual Cognition, 22(6), 770-788, doi:10.1080/13506285.2014.916772. Zhou, X. L., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1999). The nature of sublexical processing in reading Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 819-837, doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.819.
|