郭秀艳, 周楚, 周梅花. (2004). 错误记忆影响因素的实验研究. 应用心理学, 10(1), 3–8 李丹, 胡克定, 陈国鹏, 金瑜, 李眉. (1988). 瑞文测验联合型(CRT)上海市区试测报告. 心理科学, (4), 29–33 李宏英, 连榕, 翁洁. (2008). 预警、重复学习和反馈对错误记忆的影响——错误记忆的信号检测论分析. 心理科学, 31(6), 1507–1509, 1430. 毛伟宾, 杨治良. (2008). 重复学习对错误记忆通道效应的影响. 心理科学, 31(6), 1326–1329, 1325. 王协顺, 苏彦捷. (2018). 高难度材料的元记忆监测更受记忆广度的制约. 心理学探新, 38(2), 123–129 Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Gunn, D. M., & Baddeley, A. D. (2003). The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(1), 71–92, doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.71. Benjamin, A. S. (2001). On the dual effects of repetition on false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27(4), 941–947. Bodner, G. E., & Taikh, A. (2012). Reassessing the basis of the production effect in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(6), 1711–1719, doi: 10.1037/a0028466. Bodner, G. E., Taikh, A., & Fawcett, J. M. (2014). Assessing the costs and benefits of production in recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(1), 149–154, doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0485-1. Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS One, 5(6), e10729, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010729. Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309–331, doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309. Forrin, N. D., Groot, B., & MacLeod, C. M. (2016). The d-Prime directive: Assessing costs and benefits in recognition by dissociating mixed-list false alarm rates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(7), 1090–1111, doi: 10.1037/xlm0000214. Forrin, N. D., & MacLeod, C. M. (2018). This time it’s personal: The memory benefit of hearing oneself. Memory, 26(4), 574–579, doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1383434. Forrin, N. D., MacLeod, C. M., & Ozubko, J. D. (2012). Widening the boundaries of the production effect. Memory & Cognition, 40(7), 1046–1055, doi: 10.3758/s13421-01200210-8. Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 16(2), 96–101, doi: 10.3758/BF03202365. Hopkins, R. H., & Edwards, R. E. (1972). Pronunciation effects in recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(4), 534–537, doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80036-7. Jamieson, R. K., Mewhort, D. J. K., & Hockley, W. E. (2016). A computational account of the production effect: Still playing twenty questions with nature. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 70(2), 154–164, doi: 10.1037/cep0000081. Jones, A. C., & Pyc, M. A. (2014). The production effect: Costs and benefits in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(1), 300–305, doi: 10.1037/a0033337. Kell, C. A., Darquea, M., Behrens, M., Cordani, L., Keller, C., & Fuchs, S. (2017). Phonetic detail and lateralization of reading-related inner speech and of auditory and somatosensory feedback processing during overt reading. Human Brain Mapping, 38(1), 493–508, doi: 10.1002/hbm.23398. MacLeod, C. M., Gopie, N., Hourihan, K. L., Neary, K. R., & Ozubko, J. D. (2010). The production effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 671–685, doi: 10.1037/a0018785. MacLeod, C. M., Ozubko, J. D., Hourihan, K. L., & Major, J. C. (2022). The production effect is consistent over material variations: Support for the distinctiveness account. Memory. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2022.2069270. Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2004). Detection theory: A user’s guide (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press. Mama, Y., & Icht, M. (2018). Production on hold: Delaying vocal production enhances the production effect in free recall. Memory, 26(5), 589–602, doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1384496. Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1960). The distinctiveness of stimuli. Psychological Review, 67(1), 16–31, doi: 10.1037/h0042382. Oppenheim, G. M., & Dell, G. S. (2010). Motor movement matters: The flexible abstractness of inner speech. Memory & Cognition, 38(8), 1147–1160, doi: 10.3758/MC.38.8.1147. Ozubko, J. D., Hourihan, K. L., & MacLeod, C. M. (2012). Production benefits learning: The production effect endures and improves memory for text. Memory, 20(7), 717–727, doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.699070. Ozubko, J. D., & MacLeod, C. M. (2010). The production effect in memory: Evidence that distinctiveness underlies the benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(6), 1543–1547, doi: 10.1037/a0020604. Ozubko, J. D., Major, J., & MacLeod, C. M. (2014). Remembered study mode: Support for the distinctiveness account of the production effect. Memory, 22(5), 509–524, doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.800554. Peng, P., Wang, T. F., Wang, C. C., & Lin, X. (2019). A meta-analysis on the relation between fluid intelligence and reading/mathematics: Effects of tasks, age, and social economics status. Psychological Bulletin, 145(2), 189–236, doi: 10.1037/bul0000182. Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. L. (1995). Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 24–33, doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.24. Pritchard, V. E., Heron-Delaney, M., Malone, S. A., & MacLeod, C. M. (2020). The production effect improves memory in 7- to 10-year-old children. Child Development, 91(3), 901–913, doi: 10.1111/cdev.13247. Tussing, A. A., & Greene, R. L. (1999). Differential effects of repetition on true and false recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(4), 520–533, doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2636. Zhou, Y. C., & MacLeod, C. M. (2021). Production between and within: Distinctiveness and the relative magnitude of the production effect. Memory, 29(2), 168–179, doi: 10.1080/09658211.2020.1868526
|