Boy, F., Husain, M., Singh, K. D., & Sumner, P. (2010). Supplementary motor area activations in unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Experimental brain research, 206(4), 441-448.
Cole, G. G., & Kuhn, G. (2010). What the experimenter’s prime tells the observer’s brain. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(5), 1367-1376.
Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of experimental psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1737-1747.
Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 514-520.
Jaskowski, P., Biaiuńska, A., Tomanek, M., & Verleger, R. (2008). Mask-and distractor-triggered inhibitory processes in the priming of motor responses: An EEG study. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 70-85.
Klapp, S. T., & Greenberg, L. a. (2009). Temporary activation of perceptual-motor associations: a stimulus-response interpretation of automaticity. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 35(5), 1266-1285.
Klapp, S. T., & Haas, B. W. (2005). Nonconscious influence of masked stimuli on response selection is limited to concrete stimulus-response associations. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 31(1), 193-209.
Klapp, S. T., & Hinkley, L. B. (2002). The negative compatibility effect: Unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 131(2), 255-269.
Lingnau, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). The time course of response inhibition in masked priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(3), 545-557.
Liu, P., Chen, X., Dai, D., Wang, Y., & Wang, Y. (2014). A subliminal inhibitory mechanism for the negative compatibility effect: a continuous versus threshold mechanism. Experimental Brain Research, 232(7), 2305-2315.
Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming. Journal of experimental psychology. General, 133(4), 475-493.
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umilt C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31-40.
Schlaghecken, F., Birak, K. S., & Maylor, E. A. (2011). Age-related deficits in low-level inhibitory motor control. Psychology and aging, 26(4), 905-918.
Schlaghecken, F., Bowman, H., & Eimer, M. (2006). Dissociating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control in masked priming. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 32(3), 618-632.
Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2006). Active masks and active inhibition: a comment on Lleras and Enns (2004) and on Verleger, Jaskowski, Aydemir, van der Lubbe, and Groen (2004). Journal of experimental psychology: General, 135(3), 484-494.
Schlaghecken, F., Rowley, L., Sembi, S., Simmons, R., & Whitcomb, D. (2007). The negative compatibility effect: A case for self-inhibition. Advances in cognitive psychology, 3(1), 227-240.
Schlaghecken, F., & Sisman, R. (2006). Low-level motor inhibition in children: Evidence from the negative compatibility effect. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2(1), 7-19.
Sumner, P. (2008). Mask-Induced Priming and the Negative Compatibility Effect. Experimental Psychology, 55(2), 133-141.
Sumner, P., Nachev, P., Morris, P., Peters, A. M., Jackson, S. R., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2007). Human medial frontal cortex mediates unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Neuron, 54(5), 697-711. |