[1] 范小月, 王瑞明, 吴际, 林哲婷.(2012). 熟练和非熟练中英双语者不同认知控制成分的比较. 心理科学, 35(6), 1304-1308. [2] 常松, 王瑞明, 李利, 谢久书.(2013). 非熟练双语者言语产生中非目标语言的激活范围. 心理发展与教育, 29(1), 54-60. [3] Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, J., & Libnawi, A.(2010). Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81(5), 1356-1366. [4] Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S.(1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch(Eds.), Attention and performance, XV(pp. 421-452). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [5] Bialystok, E.(1986). Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child Development, 498-510. [6] Bialystok, E.(1992). Attentional control in children′s metalinguistic performance and measures of field independence. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 654-664. [7] Bialystok, E.(2011). Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 229-235. [8] Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Freedman, M.(2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 459-464. [9] Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.(2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290-203. [10] Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I.(2003). Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron, 39(4), 713-726. [11] Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.(2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278-288. [12] Carmines, E. G., & McIver, J. P.(1981). Analyzing models with unobserved variables: Analysis of covariance structures. Social Measurement: Current Issues, 65-115. [13] Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.(2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don′t. Cognition, 113(2), 135-149. [14] Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M.(2010). Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75(19), 1726-1729. [15] Cummins, J.(1987). Bilingualism, language profiency, and metalinguistic development. Childhood Bilingualism: Aspects of Linguistic, Cognitive and Social Development, 57-73. [16] Emmorey, K., Luk,G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E.(2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1201-1206. [17] Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Humphreys, G. W.(2012). Escaping capture: Bilingualism modulates distraction from working memory. Cognition, 122(1), 37-50. [18] Koch, I., Prinz, W., & Allport, A.(2005). Involuntary retrieval in alphabet-arithmetic tasks: Task-mixing and task-switching costs. Psychological Research, 69(4), 252-261. [19] Kovács, ?魣. M., & Mehler, J.(2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(16), 6556-6560. [20] Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T.(2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 416-430. [21] Los, S. A.(1996). On the origin of mixing costs: Exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials. Acta Psychologica, 94(2), 145-188. [22] Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R.(2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 362-372. [23] McDonald, R. P., & Ho, M. H. R.(2002). Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 64-82. [24] Meiran, N.(2000). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII, 377-399. [25] Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H.(2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(04), 682-691. [26] Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B.(2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(02), 253-262. [27] Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Krmer, U. M., Lorenzo-Seva, U., Festman, J., & Münte, T. F.(2011). Self-assessment of individual differences in language switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(388). [28] Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S.(1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207-231. [29] Schweizer, T. A., Ware, J., Fischer, C. E., Craik, F. I., & Bialystok, E.(2012). Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: Evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer′s disease. Cortex, 48(8), 991-996. [30] Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M.(2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(183). [31] Yim, O., & Bialystok, E.(2012). Degree of conversational code-switching enhances verbal task switching in Cantonese-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(04), 873-883.
|