李士一, 白学军, 王雨薇, 郑蕾, 孙弘进. (2017). 二维场景旋转下的背景提示效应. 心理与行为研究, 15(3), 289-294, doi:10.3969/j.issn.1672-0628.2017.03.001 肖承丽, 刘传军. (2014). 对想象环境的空间更新. 心理学报, 46(9), 1289-1300 Bar, M. (2001). Viewpoint dependency in visual object recognition does not necessarily imply viewer-centered representation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(6), 793-799, doi:10.1162/08989290152541458. Brockmole, J. R. (2008). The visual world in memory. New York:Psychology Press. Brockmole, J. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2006). Recognition and attention guidance during contextual cueing in real-world scenes:Evidence from eye movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(7), 1177-1187, doi:10.1080/17470210600665996. Burgess, N. (2006). Spatial memory:How egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 551-557, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.005. Burgess, N., Spiers, H. J., & Paleologou, E. (2004). Orientational manoeuvres in the dark:Dissociating allocentric and egocentric influences on spatial memory. Cognition, 94(2), 149-166, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.001. Chua, K. P., & Chun, M. M. (2003). Implicit scene learning is viewpoint dependent. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(1), 72-80. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing:Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 28-71, doi:10.1006/cogp.1998.0681. Diwadkar, V. A., & McNamara, T. P. (1997). Viewpoint dependence in scene recognition. Psychological Science, 8(4), 302-307, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00442.x. Jiang, Y., Song, J.-H., & Rigas, A. (2005). High-capacity spatial contextual memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(3), 524-529. Kelly, J. W., McNamara, T. P., Bodenheimer, B., Carr, T. H., & Rieser, J. J. (2009). Individual differences in using geometric and featural cues to maintain spatial orientation:Cue quantity and cue ambiguity are more important than cue type. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(1), 176-181. Kelly, J. W., Sjolund, L. A., & Sturz, B. R. (2013). Geometric cues, reference frames, and the equivalence of experienced-aligned and novel-aligned views in human spatial memory. Cognition, 126(3), 459-474, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.007. Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Beall, A. C., Chance, S. S., & Golledge, R. G. (1998). Spatial updating of self-position and orientation during real, imagined, and virtual locomotion. Psychological Science, 9(4), 293-298, doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00058. Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492-527, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492. Marchette, S. A., & Shelton, A. L. (2010). Object properties and frame of reference in spatial memory representations. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 10(1), 1-27. Mou, W. M., McNamara, T. P., Rump, B., & Xiao, C. J. (2006). Roles of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations in locomotion and reorientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1274-1290, doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1274. Reber, P. J. (2013). The neural basis of implicit learning and memory:A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research. Neuropsychologia, 51(10), 2026-2042, doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.019. Riecke, B. E., & von der Heyde, M., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005). Visual cues can be sufficient for triggering automatic, reflexlike spatial updating. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception, 2(3), 183-215, doi:10.1145/1077399.1077401. Schacter, D. L. (1992). Priming and multiple memory systems:Perceptual mechanisms of implicit memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(3), 244-256, doi:10.1162/jocn.1992.4.3.244. Shelton, A. L., & McNamara, T. P. (2001). Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory. Cognitive Psychology, 43(4), 274-310, doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0758. Shi, Z. H., Zang, X. L., Jia, L. N., Geyer, T., & Müller, H. J. (2013). Transfer of contextual cueing in full-icon display remapping. Journal of Vision, 13(3), 2-2, doi:10.1167/13.3.2. Simons, D. J., & Wang, R. F. (1998). Perceiving real-world viewpoint changes. Psychological Science, 9(4), 315-320, doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00062. Tsuchiai, T., Matsumiya, K., Kuriki, I., & Shioiri, S. (2012). Implicit learning of viewpoint-independent spatial layouts. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 207. Wang, R. F. (2003). Spatial representations and spatial updating. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 42, 109-156, doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(03)01004-1. Wang, R. F. (2012). Theories of spatial representations and reference frames:What can configuration errors tell us? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(4), 575-587. Wang, R. F. (2017). Spatial updating and common misinterpretations of spatial reference frames. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 17(3), 222-249. Wang, R. F., & Simons, D. J. (1999). Active and passive scene recognition across views. Cognition, 70(2), 191-210, doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00012-8. Wang, R. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Updating egocentric representations in human navigation. Cognition, 77(3), 215-250, doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00105-0. Wolbers, T., Hegarty, M., Büchel, C., & Loomis, J. M. (2008). Spatial updating:How the brain keeps track of changing object locations during observer motion. Nature Neuroscience, 11(10), 1223-1230, doi:10.1038/nn.2189. Xie, C. X., Li, S. Y., Tao, W. D., Wei, Y. P., & Sun, H. J. (2017). Representing spatial layout according to intrinsic frames of reference:Limitations from position regularity and instructions. Psychological Reports, 120(5), 846-869, doi:10.1177/0033294117711129. Zang, X., Shi, Z., Müller, H. J., & Conci, M. (2017). Contextual cueing in 3D visual search depends on representations in planar-, not depth-defined space. Journal of Vision, 17(5), 17-17, doi:10.1167/17.5.17. Zhao, G., Zhuang, Q., Ma, J., Tu, S., Liu, Q., & Sun, H. (2017). Contextual cueing effect in spatial layout defined by binocular disparity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1472, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01472.
|