李政汉, 杨国春, 南威治, 李琦, 刘勋. (2018). 冲突解决过程中认知控制的注意调节机制. 心理科学进展, 26(6), 966-974 夏天生, 谭玲. (2020). 情境特异比例一致性变化对冲突效应的影响. 心理科学, 43(5), 1049-1057 Blais, C., & Verguts, T. (2012). Increasing set size breaks down sequential congruency:Evidence for an associative locus of cognitive control. Acta Psychologica, 141(2), 133-139, doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.07.009. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624. Bugg, J. M., & Chanani, S. (2011). List-wide control is not entirely elusive:Evidence from picture-word Stroop. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(5), 930-936. Bugg, J. M., & Hutchison, K. A. (2013). Converging evidence for control of color-word Stroop interference at the item level. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 39(2), 433-449, doi:10.1037/a0029145. Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Chanani, S. (2011). Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 844-859, doi:10.1037/a0019957. Cao, Y., Cao, X. Y., Yue, Z. Z., & Wang, L. (2017). Temporal and spectral dynamics underlying cognitive control modulated by task-irrelevant stimulus-response learning. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(1), 158-173. De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity:A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731-750, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731. Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. A. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. Acta Psychologica, 39(6), 427-439, doi:10.1016/0001-6918(75)90041-4. Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Hessels, S. (2003). Item-specific control of automatic processes:Stroop process dissociations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 638-644. Lien, M. C., Pedersen, L., & Proctor, R. W. (2016). Stimulus-response correspondence in go-nogo and choice tasks:Are reactions altered by the presence of an irrelevant salient object? Psychological Research, 80(6), 912-934, doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0699-0. Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled:Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task. Memory & Cognition, 7(3), 166-174. Marble, J. G., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Mixing location-relevant and location-irrelevant choice-reaction tasks:Influences of location mapping on the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 26(5), 1515-1533, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.5.1515. Schmidt, J. R. (2013). The parallel episodic processing (PEP) model:Dissociating contingency and conflict adaptation in the item-specific proportion congruent paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 142(1), 119-126, doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.004. Schmidt, J. R. (2016). Temporal learning and rhythmic responding:No reduction in the proportion easy effect with variable response-stimulus intervals. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 634. Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect:Why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 514-523, doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.514. Schmidt, J. R., & Lemercier, C. (2019). Context-specific proportion congruent effects:Compound-cue contingency learning in disguise. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(5), 1119-1130, doi:10.1177/1747021818787155. Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2008). Hebbian learning of cognitive control:Dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. Psychological Review, 115(2), 518-525, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.518. Xia, T. S., Li, H., & Wang, L. (2016). Implicitly strengthened task-irrelevant stimulus-response associations modulate cognitive control:Evidence from an fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 37(2), 756-772, doi:10.1002/hbm.23064.
|