[1] Roland M, Peter T, Andreas S, et al. Digitized analysis of handwriting and drawing movements in healthy subjects: Methods, results and perspectives. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 1999, 90(2): 157~169 [2] Ruud G J, Arend W A. Van G. Advances in the study of drawing and handwriting. Human Movement Science, 2003, 22(2): 131~135 [3] Swanson H L, Jerman O. Math disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. 2006, 76(2): 249~274 [4] Remi C, Frelicot C, Courtellemont P. Automatic analysis of the structuring of children′s drawings and writing. Pattern Recognition, 2002, 35(5): 1059~1069 [5] 李蓓蕾, 林磊, 董奇. 儿童筷子使用技能特性的发展及其与学业成绩的关系. 心理科学, 2003, 26(1): 87~89 [6] Geary D C. Hoard M K, Byrd-Craven J, et al. Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2004, 88: 121~151 [7] 刘昌. 数学学习困难儿童的认知加工机制研究. 南京师大学报, 2004, 3: 81~88 [8] Richard A M著, 张忠秋等译. 运动技能学习与控制(第七版). 北京: 中国轻工业出版社, 2006 [9] Schenk T, Walther E U, Mai N. Closed-and open-loop handwriting performance in patients with multiple sclerosis. European Journal of Neurology, 2000, 7(3): 269~279 [10] Smits-Engelsman B C M, Wilson P H, Westenberg Y. Fine motor deficiencies in children with developmental coordination disorder and learning disabilities: An underlying open-loop control deficit. Human Movement Science, 2003, 22(4-5): 495~513 [11] Baddeley A. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2003, 4(10): 829~839 [12] Colom R, Rebollo I, Francisco J A, et al. Complex span tasks, simple span tasks, and cognitive abilities: A reanalysis of key studies. Memory and Cognition, 2006, 34(1): 158~171 |