孟祥斌. (2016). 项目反应时间的对数偏正态模型. 心理科学, 39(3), 727-734 朱滢. (2009). 实验心理学(第2版). 北京:北京大学出版社. Anders, R., Alario, F. X., & van Maanen, L. (2016). The shifted Wald distribution for response time data analysis. Psychological Methods, 21(3), 309-327, doi:10.1037/met0000066. Bolsinova, M., De Boeck, P., & Tijmstra, J. (2017). Modelling conditional dependence between response time and accuracy. Psychometrika, 82(4), 1126-1148, doi:10.1007/s11336-016-9537-6. Bolsinova, M., Tijmstra, J., & Molenaar, D. (2017). Response moderation models for conditional dependence between response time and response accuracy. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 70(2), 257-279, doi:10.1111/bmsp.12076. Brooks, S. P., & Gelman, A. (1998). General methods for monitoring convergence of iterative simulations. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 7(4), 434-455. Chen, H. Q., De Boeck, P., Gradya, M., Yanga, C. L., & Waldschmidta, D. (2018). Curvilinear dependency of response accuracy on response time in cognitive tests. Intelligence, 69, 16-23, doi:10.1016/j.intell.2018.04.001. Donkin, C., Little, D. R., & Houpt, J. W. (2014). Assessing the speed-accuracy trade-off effect on the capacity of information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 40(3), 1183-1202, doi:10.1037/a0035947. Giordano, A. M., McElree, B., & Carrasco, M. (2009). On the automaticity and flexibility of covert attention:A speed-accuracy trade-off analysis. Journal of Vision, 9(3), 1-10, doi:10.1167/9.3.1. Goldhammer, F., Naumann, J., & Greiff, S. (2015). More is not always better:The relation between item response and item response time in Raven's matrices. Journal of Intelligence, 3(1), 21-40, doi:10.3390/jintelligence3010021. Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). Thinking styles. In D. H. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Perspectives on individual differences. International handbook of personality and intelligence. New York:Plenum Press. Heathcote, A. (2004). Fitting Wald and ex-Wald distributions to response time data:An example using functions for the S-PLUS package. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 678-694. Heitz, R. P. (2014). The speed-accuracy tradeoff:History, physiology, methodology, and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 150. Kagan, J., Rosman, B. L., Day, D., Albert, J., & Phillips, W. (1964). Information processing in the child:Significance of analytic and reflective attitudes. Psychological Monographs:General and Applied, 78(1), 1-37, doi:10.1037/h0093830. McElree, B. (1998). Attended and non-attended states in working memory:Accessing categorized structrures. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(2), 225-252, doi:10.1006/jmla.1997.2545. McElree, B. (2000). Sentence comprehension is mediated by content-addressable memory structures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 111-123, doi:10.1023/A:1005184709695. McElree, B., & Carrasco, M. (1999). The Temporal Dynamics of Visual Search:Evidence for Parallel Processing in Feature and Conjunction Searches. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1517-1539, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1517. McElree, B., Foraker, S., & Dyer, L. (2003). Memory structures that subserve sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 67-91, doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00515-6. McElree, B., Jia, G., & Litvak, A. (2000). the time course of conceptual processing in three bilingual populations. Journal of Memory and Language, 42(2), 229-254, doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2677. Meng, X. B., Tao, J., & Chang, H. H. (2015). A conditional joint modeling approach for locally dependent item responses and response times. Journal of Educational Measurement, 52(1), 1-27, doi:10.1111/jedm.12060. Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model:Current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260-281, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.007. Reed, A. V. (1973). Speed-accuracy trade-off in recognition memory. Science, 181(4099), 574-576, doi:10.1126/science.181.4099.574. Reed, A. V. (1976). List length and the time course of recognition in immediate memory. Memory & Cognition, 4(1), 16-30. Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., & van der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B, 64, 583-616, doi:10.1111/1467-9868.00353. van der Linden, W. J. (2007). A hierarchical framework for modeling speed and accuracy on test items. Psychometrika, 72(3), 287-308, doi:10.1007/s11336-006-1478-z. van der Linden, W. J. (2009). Conceptual issues in response-time modeling. Journal of Educational Measurement, 46(3), 247-272, doi:10.1111/j.1745-3984.2009.00080.x. van der Maas, H. L. J., Molenaar, D., Maris, G., Kievit, R., & Borsboom, D. (2011). Cognitive psychology meets psychometric theory:On the relation between process models for decision making and latent variable models for individual differences. Psychological Review, 118(2), 339-356, doi:10.1037/a0022749. Wang, T. Y., & Zhang, J. W. (2006). Optimal partitioning of testing time:Theoretical properties and practical implications. Psychometrika, 71(1), 105-120, doi:10.1007/s11336-002-1013-x. Wenger, M. J., & Gibson, B. S. (2004). Using hazard functions to assess changes in processing capacity in an attentional cuing paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 30(4), 708-719, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.708. Wenger, M. J., & Townsend, J. T. (2000). Basic response time tools for studying general processing capacity in attention, perception, and cognition. The Journal of General Psychology, 127(1), 67-99, doi:10.1080/00221300009598571. Wickelgren, W. A. (1977). Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics. Acta Psychologica, 41(1), 67-85, doi:10.1016/0001-6918(77)90012-9.
|