邓颖, 徐富明, 李欧, 史燕伟, 刘程浩. (2016). 社会偏好中的框架效应. 心理科学进展, 24(4), 622–632 窦凯, 聂衍刚, 王玉洁, 黎建斌. (2014). 自我损耗促进冒险行为. 心理科学, 37(1), 150–155 段锦云, 王重鸣, 田晓明. (2010). 认知资源投入对框架效应的影响的实验研究. 应用心理学, 16(4), 307–315 段婧, 刘永芳, 何琪. (2012). 决策者角色及相关变量对风险偏好的影响. 心理学报, 44(3), 369–376 鞠成婷, 李苑, 何立国, 游旭群. (2020). 认知资源对不同感觉寻求者风险决策的调节. 心理科学, 43(4), 879–884 刘扬, 孙彦. (2014). 行为决策中框架效应研究新思路——从风险决策到跨期决策, 从言语框架到图形框架.?心理科学进展, 22(8), 1205–1217. 刘耀中, 刘敏, 彭滨. (2018). 任务框架和社会距离对风险决策的影响: 来自ERP的证据. 心理与行为研究, 16(1), 13–21 娄宇, 单雪菲, 刘宁. (2019). 权力与框架对风险寻求的影响: 解释水平的作用. 心理技术与应用, 7(4), 193–207 马钰, 肖晨洁, 车敬上, 王海侠, 李爱梅. (2020). 自我损耗降低决策理性: 心理机制与边界条件. 心理科学进展, 28(11), 1911–1925 任俊, 李瑞雪, 詹鋆, 刘迪, 林曼, 彭年强. (2014). 好人可能做出坏行为的心理学解释——基于自我控制资源损耗的研究证据. 心理学报, 46(6), 841–851 索涛, 顾本柏, 张辰炎, 刘雷. (2018). 不同特质自控者在跨期选择中自我损耗后效的差异. 心理科学, 41(6), 1443–1449 谭树华, 郭永玉. (2008). 大学生自我控制量表的修订. 中国临床心理学杂志, 16(5), 468–470 王璐璐, 李永娟. (2012). 心理疲劳与任务框架对风险决策的影响. 心理科学进展, 20(11), 1546–1550 杨玲, 王斌强, 耿银凤, 姚东伟, 曹华, 张建勋, 许琼英. (2019). 虚拟和真实金钱奖赏幅度对海洛因戒断者风险决策的影响. 心理学报, 51(4), 507–516 张慧, 陆静怡, 谢晓非. (2014). 解释水平导致的自己-他人决策差异. 北京大学学报(自然科学版), 50(6), 1124–1132 Achtziger, A., Alós-Ferrer, C., & Wagner, A. K. (2016). The impact of self-control depletion on social preferences in the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 53, 1–16, doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2015.12.005. Albrecht, K., Volz, K. G., Sutter, M., Laibson, D. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2010). What is for me is not for you: Brain correlates of intertemporal choice for self and other. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(2), 218–225. Andersson, O., Holm, H. J., Tyran, J. R., & Wengström, E. (2014). Deciding for others reduces loss aversion. Management Science, 62(1), 29–36. Barrafrem, K., & Hausfeld, J. (2020). Tracing risky decisions for oneself and others: The role of intuition and deliberation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 77, 102188, doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2019.102188. Benjamin, A. M., & Robbins, S. J. (2007). The role of framing effects in performance on the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Personality and Individual Differences, 43(2), 221–230. Bogg, T., Lasecki, L., & Vo, P. T. (2016). School investment, drinking motives, and high-risk, high-reward partying decisions mediate the relationship between trait self-control and alcohol consumption among college drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 77(1), 133–142, doi: 10.15288/jsad.2016.77.133. Bornovalova, M. A., Cashman-Rolls, A., O’Donnell, J. M., Ettinger, K., Richards, J. B., deWit, H., & Lejuez, C. W. (2009). Risk taking differences on a behavioral task as a function of potential reward/loss magnitude and individual differences in impulsivity and sensation seeking. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93(3), 258–262. Brewer, G. A., Lau, K. K. H., Wingert, K. M., Ball, B. H., & Blais, C. (2017). Examining depletion theories under conditions of within-task transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(7), 988–1008, doi: 10.1037/xge0000290. Brown, D. M. Y., Graham, J. D., Innes, K. I., Harris, S., Flemington, A., & Bray, S. R. (2020). Effects of prior cognitive exertion on physical performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Medicine, 50(3), 497–529. Campbell, J. A., Samartgis, J. R., & Crowe, S. F. (2013). Impaired decision making on the Balloon Analogue Risk Task as a result of long-term alcohol use. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35(10), 1071–1081, doi: 10.1080/13803395.2013.856382. Claus, E. D., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Magnan, R. E., Montanaro, E., Hutchison, K. E., & Bryan, A. D. (2017). Neural mechanisms of risky decision making in adolescents reporting frequent alcohol and/or marijuana use. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 12(2), 564–576. De Haan, T., & van Veldhuizen, R. (2015). Willpower depletion and framing effects. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 117, 47–61. De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science, 313(5787), 684–687, doi: 10.1126/science.1128356. Duckworth, A. L., Milkman, K. L., & Laibson, D. (2018). Beyond willpower: Strategies for reducing failures of self-control. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(3), 102–129. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. Fernandez-Duque, D., & Wifall, T. (2007). Actor/observer asymmetry in risky decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(1), 1–8. Freeman, N., & Muraven, M. (2010). Self-control depletion leads to increased risk taking. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(2), 175–181, doi: 10.1177/1948550609360421. Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(3), 351–367, doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351. Gao, W., Zhou, Y., Tao, T., Yu, Y., & Wang, L. (2019). Ego depletion in the relationship between behavior inhibition and loss aversion. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 47(4), e7713. Gerhardt, H., Schildberg-Hörisch, H., & Willrodt, J. (2017). Does self-control depletion affect risk attitudes. European Economic Review, 100, 463–487, doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2017.09.004. Guo, H., Song, H., Liu, Y., Xu, K., & Shen, H. (2019). Social distance modulates the process of uncertain decision-making: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 12, 701–714, doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S210910. Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 495–525, doi: 10.1037/a0019486. Helfinstein, S. M., Schonberg, T., Congdon, E., Karlsgodt, K. H., Mumford, J. A., Sabb, F. W., … Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Predicting risky choices from brain activity patterns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(7), 2470–2475. Hsee, C. K., & Weber, E. U. (1997). A fundamental prediction error: Self-others discrepancies in risk preference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(1), 45–53. Jung, D., Sul, S., & Kim, H. (2013). Dissociable neural processes underlying risky decisions for self versus other. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 15. Kim, J., Lee, S., & Rua, T. (2015). Feeling depleted and powerless: The construal-level mechanism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(4), 599–609, doi: 10.1177/0146167215574993. Kostek, J., & Ashrafioun, L. (2014). Tired winners: The effects of cognitive resources and prior winning on risky decision making. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30(2), 423–434, doi: 10.1007/s10899-013-9365-x. Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., … Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 75–84. Lermer, E., Streicher, B., Sachs, R., Raue, M., & Frey, D. (2016). Thinking concretely increases the perceived likelihood of risks: The effect of construal level on risk estimation. Risk Analysis, 36(3), 623–637. Lyu, R. W., Lai, C. Q., & Liu, J. (2017). The role of ego depletion in goal pursuit: A construal-level perspective. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 45(7), 1143–1156. Pang, B., Otto, A. R., & Worthy, D. A. (2015). Self-control moderates decision-making behavior when minimizing losses versus maximizing gains. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 28(2), 176–187. Polman, E., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Decision fatigue, choosing for others, and self-construal. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(5), 471–478, doi: 10.1177/1948550616639648. Pronin, E., Olivola, C. Y., & Kennedy, K. A. (2008). Doing unto future selves as you would do unto others: Psychological distance and decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(2), 224–236, doi: 10.1177/0146167207310023. Shen, Q., Jin, J., & Ma, Q. G. (2013). The sweet side of inequality: How advantageous status modulates empathic response to others’ gains and losses. Behavioural Brain Research, 256, 609–617. Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 311–322. Sun, Q. Z., Jiang, T., Zhang, J., Wu, Q. Y., Zhao, L., & Hu, F. P. (2020). Riskier for me or for others? The role of domain and probability in self-other differences, in risky decision-making. The Journal of General Psychology, 147(2), 169–185, doi: 10.1080/00221309.2019.1664388. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403–421, doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403. Unger, A., & Stahlberg, D. (2011). Ego-depletion and risk behavior: Too exhausted to take a risk. Social Psychology, 42(1), 28–38. Yan, X. H. (2014). The effect of self-control resource on risk preference. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 42(8), 1335–1344, doi: 10.2224/sbp.2014.42.8.1335. Zhang, X. Y., Li, S. J., Liu, Y. F., Chen, X. Y., Shang, X. S., Qi, F. Z., .. Chen, J. (2019). Gain-loss situation modulates neural responses to self-other decision making under risk. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 632.
|