Studies of Psychology and Behavior ›› 2023, Vol. 21 ›› Issue (1): 20-27.DOI: 10.12139/j.1672-0628.2023.01.004
• ? • Previous Articles Next Articles
Zebo LAN1,2, Ziming SONG3, Kun JIANG4, Guoli YAN1,*()
Received:
2022-03-15
Online:
2023-01-20
Published:
2023-01-20
Contact:
Guoli YAN
通讯作者:
闫国利
基金资助:
CLC Number:
Zebo LAN, Ziming SONG, Kun JIANG, Guoli YAN. Tongue Twister Effect of Chinese Reading in Deaf College Students: The Influence of Oral Experience and Reading Ability[J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2023, 21(1): 20-27.
兰泽波, 宋子明, 姜琨, 闫国利. 听障大学生汉语阅读中的绕口令效应:口语经验和阅读能力的影响[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(1): 20-27.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://psybeh.tjnu.edu.cn/EN/10.12139/j.1672-0628.2023.01.004
人口学 变量 | OHRH(n=25) | OLRH(n=24) | OLRL(n=14) | F |
年龄(岁) | 22.34(0.28) | 22.91(0.25) | 22.34(0.37) | 1.34 |
口语使用比例 | 0.75(0.03) | 0.19(0.02) | 0.10(0.04) | 148.30*** |
手语使用比例 | 0.25(0.03) | 0.81(0.02) | 0.90(0.04) | 148.30*** |
口语水平 | 0.64(0.03) | 0.32(0.03) | 0.14(0.05) | 51.66*** |
手语水平 | 0.49(0.04) | 0.72(0.02) | 0.63(0.03) | 12.60*** |
智力(IQ) | 113.00(2.53) | 111.79(2.92) | 112.75(4.39) | 0.05 |
阅读流畅性(字/分钟) | 453.40(20.87) | 444.53(19.91) | 264.05(13.88) | 21.76*** |
听力损失程度(dB) | 95.64(3.04) | 102.29(2.61) | 107.36(3.61) | 3.40* |
听障时间(岁) | 1.18(0.22) | 1.02(0.17) | 0.82(0.29) | 0.59 |
人口学 变量 | OHRH(n=25) | OLRH(n=24) | OLRL(n=14) | F |
年龄(岁) | 22.34(0.28) | 22.91(0.25) | 22.34(0.37) | 1.34 |
口语使用比例 | 0.75(0.03) | 0.19(0.02) | 0.10(0.04) | 148.30*** |
手语使用比例 | 0.25(0.03) | 0.81(0.02) | 0.90(0.04) | 148.30*** |
口语水平 | 0.64(0.03) | 0.32(0.03) | 0.14(0.05) | 51.66*** |
手语水平 | 0.49(0.04) | 0.72(0.02) | 0.63(0.03) | 12.60*** |
智力(IQ) | 113.00(2.53) | 111.79(2.92) | 112.75(4.39) | 0.05 |
阅读流畅性(字/分钟) | 453.40(20.87) | 444.53(19.91) | 264.05(13.88) | 21.76*** |
听力损失程度(dB) | 95.64(3.04) | 102.29(2.61) | 107.36(3.61) | 3.40* |
听障时间(岁) | 1.18(0.22) | 1.02(0.17) | 0.82(0.29) | 0.59 |
句子类型 | 示例 |
绕口令 | 史老师时常讲时事而石老师时常读报纸。 |
控制句 | 王老师经常讲政治而何老师经常读报纸。 |
句子类型 | 示例 |
绕口令 | 史老师时常讲时事而石老师时常读报纸。 |
控制句 | 王老师经常讲政治而何老师经常读报纸。 |
眼动指标 | OHRH | OLRH | OLRL | |||||
正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | |||
阅读速度(字/分钟) | 189(3) | 160(3) | 196(4) | 168(4) | 160(4) | 148(4) | ||
平均注视时间(ms) | 243(1) | 248(1) | 262(2) | 269(2) | 247(2) | 251(1) | ||
注视次数(次) | 30.80(0.65) | 35.97(0.79) | 27.72(0.58) | 31.59(0.66) | 35.63(1.04) | 38.00(1.10) | ||
回视次数(次) | 9.25(0.25) | 11.01(0.29) | 8.60(0.25) | 9.78(0.28) | 12.30(0.44) | 12.91(0.46) |
眼动指标 | OHRH | OLRH | OLRL | |||||
正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | |||
阅读速度(字/分钟) | 189(3) | 160(3) | 196(4) | 168(4) | 160(4) | 148(4) | ||
平均注视时间(ms) | 243(1) | 248(1) | 262(2) | 269(2) | 247(2) | 251(1) | ||
注视次数(次) | 30.80(0.65) | 35.97(0.79) | 27.72(0.58) | 31.59(0.66) | 35.63(1.04) | 38.00(1.10) | ||
回视次数(次) | 9.25(0.25) | 11.01(0.29) | 8.60(0.25) | 9.78(0.28) | 12.30(0.44) | 12.91(0.46) |
效应 | 阅读速度 | 平均注视时间 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 5.00 | 0.06 | 90.25 | <0.001 | [4.89, 5.11] | 5.52 | 0.01 | 368.96 | <0.001 | [5.49, 5.55] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | 0.05 | 0.11 | 0.42 | 0.68 | [−0.17, 0.26] | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.29 | <0.05 | [0.01, 0.14] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | −0.17 | 0.13 | −1.31 | 0.20 | [−0.43, 0.09] | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.53 | 0.13 | [−0.13, 0.02] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | −0.14 | 0.02 | −6.39 | <0.001 | [−0.18, −0.10] | 0.02 | 0.00 | 5.30 | <0.001 | [0.01, 0.03] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | 0.05 | 0.03 | 1.73 | 0.09 | [−0.01, 0.11] | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.14 | 0.89 | [−0.01, 0.02] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | 0.07 | 0.04 | 1.73 | 0.09 | [−0.01, 0.14] | −0.00 | 0.01 | −0.56 | 0.58 | [−0.02, 0.01] | |
效应 | 注视次数 | 回视次数 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 3.41 | 0.05 | 62.24 | <0.001 | [3.30, 3.52] | 2.22 | 0.07 | 32.00 | <0.001 | [2.08, 2.35] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | −0.11 | 0.11 | −1.04 | 0.30 | [−0.32, 0.10] | −0.12 | 0.14 | −0.89 | 0.37 | [−0.39, 0.15] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | 0.21 | 0.13 | 1.65 | 0.10 | [−0.04, 0.47] | 0.36 | 0.17 | 2.18 | <0.05 | [0.04, 0.69] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | 0.12 | 0.02 | 5.05 | <0.001 | [0.08, 0.17] | 0.14 | 0.03 | 4.11 | <0.001 | [0.07, 0.20] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH) × (绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.51 | 0.13 | [−0.14, 0.02] | −0.09 | 0.05 | −1.57 | 0.12 | [−0.19, 0.02] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH) × (绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.05 | 0.04 | −1.17 | 0.24 | [−0.13, 0.03] | −0.08 | 0.06 | −1.50 | 0.13 | [−0.19, 0.03] |
效应 | 阅读速度 | 平均注视时间 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 5.00 | 0.06 | 90.25 | <0.001 | [4.89, 5.11] | 5.52 | 0.01 | 368.96 | <0.001 | [5.49, 5.55] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | 0.05 | 0.11 | 0.42 | 0.68 | [−0.17, 0.26] | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.29 | <0.05 | [0.01, 0.14] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | −0.17 | 0.13 | −1.31 | 0.20 | [−0.43, 0.09] | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.53 | 0.13 | [−0.13, 0.02] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | −0.14 | 0.02 | −6.39 | <0.001 | [−0.18, −0.10] | 0.02 | 0.00 | 5.30 | <0.001 | [0.01, 0.03] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | 0.05 | 0.03 | 1.73 | 0.09 | [−0.01, 0.11] | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.14 | 0.89 | [−0.01, 0.02] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | 0.07 | 0.04 | 1.73 | 0.09 | [−0.01, 0.14] | −0.00 | 0.01 | −0.56 | 0.58 | [−0.02, 0.01] | |
效应 | 注视次数 | 回视次数 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 3.41 | 0.05 | 62.24 | <0.001 | [3.30, 3.52] | 2.22 | 0.07 | 32.00 | <0.001 | [2.08, 2.35] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | −0.11 | 0.11 | −1.04 | 0.30 | [−0.32, 0.10] | −0.12 | 0.14 | −0.89 | 0.37 | [−0.39, 0.15] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | 0.21 | 0.13 | 1.65 | 0.10 | [−0.04, 0.47] | 0.36 | 0.17 | 2.18 | <0.05 | [0.04, 0.69] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | 0.12 | 0.02 | 5.05 | <0.001 | [0.08, 0.17] | 0.14 | 0.03 | 4.11 | <0.001 | [0.07, 0.20] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH) × (绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.51 | 0.13 | [−0.14, 0.02] | −0.09 | 0.05 | −1.57 | 0.12 | [−0.19, 0.02] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH) × (绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.05 | 0.04 | −1.17 | 0.24 | [−0.13, 0.03] | −0.08 | 0.06 | −1.50 | 0.13 | [−0.19, 0.03] |
眼动指标 | OHRH | OLRH | OLRL | |||||
正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | |||
首次注视时间(ms) | 252(1) | 257(1) | 272(2) | 280(2) | 255(2) | 256(2) | ||
凝视时间(ms) | 267(1) | 276(2) | 286(2) | 340(2) | 271(2) | 274(1) | ||
回视路径时间(ms) | 587(12) | 658(14) | 617(14) | 685(15) | 728(22) | 728(21) | ||
总注视时间(ms) | 490(4) | 555(4) | 489(4) | 547(4) | 549(6) | 580(6) |
眼动指标 | OHRH | OLRH | OLRL | |||||
正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | 正常句 | 绕口令 | |||
首次注视时间(ms) | 252(1) | 257(1) | 272(2) | 280(2) | 255(2) | 256(2) | ||
凝视时间(ms) | 267(1) | 276(2) | 286(2) | 340(2) | 271(2) | 274(1) | ||
回视路径时间(ms) | 587(12) | 658(14) | 617(14) | 685(15) | 728(22) | 728(21) | ||
总注视时间(ms) | 490(4) | 555(4) | 489(4) | 547(4) | 549(6) | 580(6) |
效应 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 5.49 | 0.02 | 337.65 | <0.001 | [5.46, 5.52] | 5.54 | 0.02 | 339.74 | <0.001 | [5.51, 5.57] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.11 | <0.05 | [0.01, 0.14] | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.08 | <0.05 | [0.00, 0.14] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.55 | 0.13 | [−0.15, 0.02] | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.55 | 0.13 | [−0.14, 0.02] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 3.87 | <0.001 | [0.01, 0.02] | 0.03 | 0.00 | 5.29 | <0.001 | [0.02, 0.03] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.49 | 0.63 | [−0.01, 0.02] | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.75 | 0.46 | [−0.01, 0.03] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.02 | 0.01 | −1.67 | 0.10 | [−0.04, 0.00] | −0.02 | 0.01 | −1.95 | 0.06 | [−0.05, 0.00] | |
效应 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 5.99 | 0.03 | 235.52 | <0.001 | [5.94, 6.04] | 6.02 | 0.04 | 166.57 | <0.001 | [5.95, 6.09] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | 0.05 | 0.05 | 1.05 | 0.30 | [−0.05, 0.15] | 0.01 | 0.07 | 0.11 | 0.92 | [−0.13, 0.14] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | 0.04 | 0.06 | 0.64 | 0.52 | [−0.08, 0.16] | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.87 | 0.39 | [−0.09, 0.24] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | 0.06 | 0.01 | 6.51 | <0.001 | [0.04, 0.08] | 0.09 | 0.02 | 5.60 | <0.001 | [0.06, 0.13] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.68 | 0.50 | [−0.05, 0.03] | −0.03 | 0.02 | −1.51 | 0.14 | [−0.08, 0.01] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.05 | 0.02 | −1.99 | 0.05 | [−0.10, −0.00] | −0.03 | 0.03 | −1.13 | 0.26 | [−0.09, 0.02] |
效应 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 5.49 | 0.02 | 337.65 | <0.001 | [5.46, 5.52] | 5.54 | 0.02 | 339.74 | <0.001 | [5.51, 5.57] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.11 | <0.05 | [0.01, 0.14] | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.08 | <0.05 | [0.00, 0.14] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.55 | 0.13 | [−0.15, 0.02] | −0.06 | 0.04 | −1.55 | 0.13 | [−0.14, 0.02] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 3.87 | <0.001 | [0.01, 0.02] | 0.03 | 0.00 | 5.29 | <0.001 | [0.02, 0.03] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.49 | 0.63 | [−0.01, 0.02] | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.75 | 0.46 | [−0.01, 0.03] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.02 | 0.01 | −1.67 | 0.10 | [−0.04, 0.00] | −0.02 | 0.01 | −1.95 | 0.06 | [−0.05, 0.00] | |
效应 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | |||||||||
b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | b | SE | t | p | 95%CI | ||
截距 | 5.99 | 0.03 | 235.52 | <0.001 | [5.94, 6.04] | 6.02 | 0.04 | 166.57 | <0.001 | [5.95, 6.09] | |
OLRH vs. OHRH | 0.05 | 0.05 | 1.05 | 0.30 | [−0.05, 0.15] | 0.01 | 0.07 | 0.11 | 0.92 | [−0.13, 0.14] | |
OLRL vs. OLRH | 0.04 | 0.06 | 0.64 | 0.52 | [−0.08, 0.16] | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.87 | 0.39 | [−0.09, 0.24] | |
绕口令 vs. 正常句 | 0.06 | 0.01 | 6.51 | <0.001 | [0.04, 0.08] | 0.09 | 0.02 | 5.60 | <0.001 | [0.06, 0.13] | |
(OLRH vs. OHRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.68 | 0.50 | [−0.05, 0.03] | −0.03 | 0.02 | −1.51 | 0.14 | [−0.08, 0.01] | |
(OLRL vs. OLRH)×(绕口令 vs. 正常句) | −0.05 | 0.02 | −1.99 | 0.05 | [−0.10, −0.00] | −0.03 | 0.03 | −1.13 | 0.26 | [−0.09, 0.02] |
程亚华, 伍新春 小学一年级阅读流畅性对二、三年级阅读理解的预测. 心理发展与教育, 2018, 34 (3): 314- 321. | |
兰泽波, 梁晓伟, 王正光, 姜琨, 孟珠, 闫国利 听障大学生句子阅读中语音加工的眼动研究. 心理科学, 2020, 43 (4): 997- 1003. | |
兰泽波, 林梅, 宋子明, 孟珠, 姜琨, 闫国利 听障大学生阅读中的语音激活: 来自绕口令效应的证据. 心理科学, 2022, 45 (2): 491- 497. | |
李丹, 胡克定, 陈国鹏, 金瑜, 李眉 瑞文测验联合型(CRT)上海市区试测报告. 心理科学通讯, 1988, (4): 29- 33. | |
王克瑞, 杜丽华. (2012). 播音员主持人训练手册: 绕口令. 北京: 中国广播电视出版社. | |
王志强, 王雁. (2016). 语音与字形在高频汉字识别中的作用——基于听障大学生与健听大学生的比较研究. 中国特殊教育, (11), 26–31, 43. | |
闫国利, 兰泽波, 孟珠, 王影超, 王正光 听障者语音编码的研究范式述评. 心理科学, 2019, 42 (2): 500- 505. | |
闫国利, 宋子明, 刘璐, 孟珠 绕口令效应对汉语阅读影响的眼动研究. 心理科学, 2017, 40 (2): 290- 295. | |
昝飞, 谭和平 聋生汉字识别的同音判断与启动效应实验研究. 心理科学, 2005, 28 (5): 1089- 1095. | |
张喜燕. (2014). 绕口令小辞典. 北京: 商务印书馆国际有限公司. | |
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. Retrieved December 1, 2021, from arXiv:1506.04967v2 | |
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 2014, 67 (1): 1- 48. | |
Bélanger, N. N., Baum, S. R., & Mayberry, R. I. (2012). Reading difficulties in adult deaf readers of French: Phonological codes, not guilty! Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(3), 263–285. | |
Bélanger, N. N., Mayberry, R. I., & Rayner, K. Orthographic and phonological preview benefits: Parafoveal processing in skilled and less-skilled deaf readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2013, 66 (11): 2237- 2252.
DOI |
|
Blythe, H. I., Dickins, J. H., Kennedy, C. R., & Liversedge, S. P. Phonological processing during silent reading in teenagers who are deaf/hard of hearing: An eye movement investigation. Developmental Science, 2018, 21 (5): e12643.
DOI |
|
Elliott, E. A., Braun, M., Kuhlmann, M., & Jacobs, A. M. A dual-route cascaded model of reading by deaf adults: Evidence for grapheme to viseme conversion. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2012, 17 (2): 227- 243.
DOI |
|
Fariña, N., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. Phonological and orthographic coding in deaf skilled readers. Cognition, 2017, 168, 27- 33.
DOI |
|
Friesen, D. C., & Joanisse, M. F. Homophone effects in deaf readers: Evidence from lexical decision. Reading and Writing, 2012, 25 (2): 375- 388.
DOI |
|
Hanson, V. L., Goodell, E. W., & Perfetti C. A. Tongue-twister effects in the silent reading of hearing and deaf college students. Journal of Memory and Language, 1991, 30 (3): 319- 330.
DOI |
|
Harris, M., & Moreno, C. Speech reading and learning to read: A comparison of 8-year-old profoundly deaf children with good and poor reading ability. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2006, 11 (2): 189- 201. | |
Harris, M., Terlektsi, E., & Kyle, F. E. Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading for deaf and hearing children in primary school. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2017, 22 (2): 233- 242.
DOI |
|
Hermans, D., Knoors, H., Ormel, E., & Verhoeven, L. Modeling reading vocabulary learning in deaf children in bilingual education programs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2008, 13 (2): 155- 174.
DOI |
|
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 2017, 82 (13): 1- 26. | |
Kyle, F. E. (2015). Research methods in studying reading and literacy development in deaf children who sign. In E. Orfanidou, B. Woll, & G. Morgan (Eds.), Research methods in sign language studies: A practical guide (pp. 300–318). Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley. | |
Li, D. G., & Lin, K. DHH students’ phoneme repetition awareness in sentence reading. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2020, 25 (4): 505- 516.
DOI |
|
Li, P., Zhang, F., Yu, A. Y., & Zhao, X. W. Language History Questionnaire (LHQ3): An enhanced tool for assessing multilingual experience. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2020, 23 (5): 938- 944.
DOI |
|
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. | |
McCutchen, D., & Perfetti, C. A. The visual tongue-twister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1982, 21 (6): 672- 687.
DOI |
|
Meteyard, L., & Davies, R. A. I. Best practice guidance for linear mixed-effects models in psychological science. Journal of Memory and Language, 2020, 112, 104092.
DOI |
|
Morey, R. D., Rouder, J. N., & Jamil, X. (2015). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. Version 0.9.12. Retrieved December 1, 2021, from http://bayesfactorpcl.r-forge.r-project.org | |
Ormel, E., Hermans, D., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L. Cross-language effects in written word recognition: The case of bilingual deaf children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2012, 15 (2): 288- 303.
DOI |
|
Perfetti, C. A., & Sandak, R. Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2000, 5 (1): 32- 50.
DOI |
|
Thierfelder, P., Wigglesworth, G., & Tang, G. Orthographic and phonological activation in Hong Kong deaf readers: An eye-tracking study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2020, 73 (12): 2217- 2235.
DOI |
|
Transler, C., & Reitsma, P. Phonological coding in reading of deaf children: Pseudohomophone effects in lexical decision. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2005, 23 (4): 525- 542.
DOI |
|
Yan, G. L., Lan, Z. B., Meng, Z., Wang, Y. C., & Benson, V. Phonological coding during sentence reading in Chinese deaf readers: An eye-tracking study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2021, 25 (4): 287- 303.
DOI |
|
Yao, P. P., Staub, A. & Li, X. S. Predictability eliminates neighborhood effects during Chinese sentence reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2022, 29 (2): 243- 252. | |
Zhang, S., & Perfetti, C. A. (1993). The tongue-twister effect in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1082–1093. | |
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Lété, B., & Grainger, J. Orthographic and phonological contributions to reading development: Tracking developmental trajectories using masked priming. Developmental Psychology, 2014, 50 (4): 1026- 1036.
DOI |
[1] | YANG Xue, LEI Jianghua. The Regulatory Role of Reading Ability in Phonological Coding of Hearing-Impaired College Students [J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2021, 19(5): 599-605. |
[2] | LIU Yueyue, WANG Ruiming, LI Li, WU Limu. Phonological Activation of Non-Target Language During Unbalanced Chinese-English Bilinguals' Language Production [J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2020, 18(1): 39-44. |
[3] | JIANG Kun, TAO Jiayu, MENG Zhu, LIU Tuo, LIU Haihan, LI Chengfeng. Deaf College Student Adjustment Scale [J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2018, 16(6): 854-859. |
[4] | ZHANG Yuzhi, ZHANG Jijia. Effects of Semantic Transparency and Structure Type of the Phonogram on Phonological Activation of Semantic Radicals [J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2018, 16(5): 612-617. |
[5] | TAN Ke, MA Jie, LIAN Kunyu, GUO Zhiying, BAI Xuejun. Verbal Working Memory and Reading Ability of Chinese Children with Double-deficit Developmental Dyslexia [J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2018, 16(3): 308-314,354. |
[6] | Wen Suxia;Rabigul Bakri. [J]. , 2009, 7(2): 134-136. |
[7] | Zhou Xiaolin,Qu Yanxuan,Zhuang Jie. THE RELATIVE TIME COURSE OF PHONOLOGICAL AND SEMANTIC ACTIVATION IN READING CHINESE: FURTHER EVIDENCE [J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2003, 1(4): 241-247. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||