官群.(2007). 具身认知观对语言理解的新诠释-心理模拟: 语言理解的一种手段.心理科学, 30(5), 1252-1256. 许先文.(2009). 语言具身认知原理及英语理解的心理模型. 杭州师范大学学报, 6(1), 114-116. 叶浩生. (2010). 具身认知:认知心理学的新取向. 心理科学进展, 18(5), 705-710. Badets, A., & Pesenti, M. (2010) Creating number semantics through finger movement perception. Cognition, 115(1), 46-53. Badets, A., Andres, M, Di Luca, S., & Pesenti, M. (2007). Number magnitude potentiates action judgements. Experimental Brain Research, 180(3), 525-534. Borghi, A. M, Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2004). Putting words in perspective. Memory and Cognition, 32, 863-873. Bub, D. N., Masson, M. E. J., & Cree, G. S. (2008). Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words. Cognition, 106(1), 27-58. Chiou, R.Y.C., Chang, E.C., Tzeng, O.J.L., & Wu, D.H. (2009). The common magnitude code underlying numerical and size processing for action but not for perception. Experimental Brain Research, 194(4), 53-562. Critchley, M. (1953). The Parietal Lobes. Hafner Press. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S.,& Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396. Ferreira, F., &Clifton, C. (1986). The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 25(3), 348-368. Fischer, M.H. (2006). The future for SNARC could be stark. Cortex, 42(8), 1066-1068. Fischer, M.H., Castel, A.D., Dodd, M.D., &Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555-556. Glenberg, A. M., Becker, R., Klotzer, S., Kolkano, L., Muller, S., & Rinck, M. (2009). Episodic affordances contribute to language comprehension. Language and Cognition, 1, 113-135. Glenberg, A.M.,& Kaschak, M.P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558-5645. Guan, C. Q. Meng, W-J, Yao, R., & Glenberg, A. (2013). Motor system contributions to the comprehension of abstract language. Brain & Language, in press. Hubbard, E.M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(6), 435-448. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: The University of Chicago press. Langston, W. (2002). Violating orientational metaphors slows reading. Discourse Processes, 34, 281-310. Langston, W., Kramer, D.C., & Glenberg, A.M. (1998). The representation of space in mental models derived from text. Memory and Cognition 26(2), 247-262. Micheal L. Anderson. (2004). Embodied Cognition: A Field Guide, Artificial Intelligence, www . elsevier com/ locat e/ art int. Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Funes, M.J., &Lupianez, J. (2010). Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 36(1), 17-24. Pickering, M. J., & Garrod. S. (2007). Do people use language production to make prediction during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11(3), 105-110. Rizzolati, G., Sinigaglia, C., & Anderson, F. (2008). Mirrors in the brain: How our minds share actions and emotions. Oxford University Press. Speer, N.K., & Zacks, J.A. (2005). Temporal changes as event boundaries: Processing and memory consequences of narrative time shifts. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(1), 125-140. Walsh,V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(11), 483-488. Zwaan, R.A., & Taylor, L.J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in languae comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1-11. |