姚树霞, 杨东, 齐森青, 雷燕, Ding, C. (2012). 视觉空间注意研究中的N2pc成分述评. 心理科学进展, 20(3), 53-63 Ásgeirsson, Á. G., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2017). No arousal-biased competition in focused visuospatial attention. Cognition, 168, 191-204 Berggren, N., & Eimer, M. (2018). Object-based target templates guide attention during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(9), 1368-1382, doi: 10.1037/xhp0000541. Brisson, B., Robitaille, N., & Jolicoeur, P. (2007). Stimulus intensity affects the latency but not the amplitude of the N2pc. Neuroreport, 18(15), 1627-1630, doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f0b559. Brouwer, A. M., Hogervorst, M. A., Oudejans, B., Ries, A. J., & Touryan, J. (2017). EEG and eye tracking signatures of target encoding during structured visual search. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 264, doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00264. Chun,M. M., & Wolfe, J. M. (1996). Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present? Cognitive Psychology, 30(1), 39-78, doi: 10.1006/cogp.1996.0002. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193-222, doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. Desmedt, J. E. (1980). P300 in serial tasks: An essential post-decision closure mechanism. Progress in Brain Research, 54, 682-686, doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61690-8. Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99(3), 225-234, doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9. Gao, Z. F., Yu, S. X., Zhu, C. F., Shui, R. D., Weng, X. C., Li, P., & Shen, M. W. (2016). Object-based encoding in visual working memory: Evidence from memory-driven attentional capture. Scientific Reports, 6, 22822, doi: 10.1038/srep22822. Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2009). The role of visual working memory (VWM) in the control of gaze during visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 936-949. Kim, S., & Yang, S. C. (2016). Memory-based attentional capture by colour and shape contents in visual working memory. Visual Cognition, 24(1), 1-12, doi: 10.1080/13506285.2016.1213000. Kumar, S., Soto, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Electrophysiological evidence for attentional guidance by the contents of working memory. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(2), 307-317, doi: 10.1111/ejn.2009.30.issue-2. Liu, Q., Lin, S. Y., Zhao, G., & Roberson, D. (2016). The effect of modulating top-down attention deployment on the N2pc/PCN. Biological Psychology, 117, 187-193, doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.04.004. Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(5), 1000-1014, doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1000. Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychological Bulletin, 131(4), 510-532, doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510. Olivers, C. N. L., Meijer, F., & Theeuwes, J. (2006). Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: visual working memory content affects visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(5), 1243-1265, doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.5.1243. Olson, I. R., Moore, K. S., & Drowos, D. B. (2008). The contents of visual memory are only partly under volitional control. Memory & Cognition, 36(7), 1360-1369. Proulx, M. J., & Egeth, H. E. (2008). Biased competition and visual search: The role of luminance and size contrast. Psychological Research, 72(1), 106-113. Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Blanco, M. J. (2005). Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 248-261, doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.248. Tan, J. F., Zhao, Y. F., Wu, S. S., Wang, L. J., Hitchman, G., & Tian, X., et al. (2014). The temporal dynamics of visual working memory guidance of selective attention. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(8), 345. Telling, A. L., Kumar, S., Meyer, A. S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2010). Electrophysiological evidence of semantic interference in visual search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2212-2225, doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21348. Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature, 400(6747), 867-869, doi: 10.1038/23698. Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2003). Serial deployment of attention during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 121-138, doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.121.
|