陈俊, 林少惠, 张积家. (2011). 汕话-普通话双言者的词汇习得年龄效应. 心理学报, 43, 111-122 李莹丽, 吴思娜, 刘丽虹. (2012). 双语经验对认知能力的影响. 心理科学进展, 20, 995-1002 王婷, 王丹, 张积家, 崔健爱. (2017). "各说各话"的语言经验对景颇族大学生执行功能的影响. 心理学报, 49, 1392-1403 张积家. (2001). 论儿童言语交际能力的结构及发展. 社会心理科学, (1), 20-24 张积家, 张凤玲. (2010). 双语和双言对图片命名和分类的不对称影响. 心理学报, 42, 452-466 Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, J., & Libnawi, A. (2010). Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81, 1356-1366, doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01477.x. Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116, 953-970, doi:10.1037/a0016923. Apperly, I. A., Warren, F., Andrews, B. J., Grant, J., & Todd, S. (2011). Developmental continuity in theory of mind:Speed and accuracy of belief-desire reasoning in children and adults. Child Development, 82, 1691-1703, doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01635.x. Back, E., & Apperly, I. A. (2010). Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription. Cognition, 115, 54-70, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.008. Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. J. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110-118, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006. Bialystok, E. (2007). Cognitive effects of bilingualism:How linguistic experience leads to cognitive change. The International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 210-223, doi:10.2167/beb441.0. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task:Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24, 40-49, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044. Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2011). Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118, 245-257, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.012. Butterfill, S. A., & Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind & Language, 28, 606-637. Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague/Paris:Mouton. Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377-395, doi:10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4. Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49, 222-251, doi:10.3102/00346543049002222. Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2005). Theory of mind. Current Biology, 15, R644-R646, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.041. Garnham, W. A., & Perner, J. (2001). Actions really do speak louder than words-but only implicitly:Young children's understanding of false belief in action. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 413-432, doi:10.1348/026151001166182. Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism:Language and Cognition, 6, 1-15, doi:10.1017/S1366728903001007. Hymes, D. H. (1972). On communicative competence. In J. B. Pride & J. Holmes (Eds.), Sociolinguistics:Selected readings (pp. 269-293). Harmondsworth:Penguin. Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense:Susceptibility to others'beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830-1834, doi:10.1126/science.1190792. Low, J. (2010). Preschoolers'implicit and explicit false-belief understanding:Relations with complex syntactical mastery. Child Development, 81, 597-615, doi:10.1111/(ISSN)1467-8624. Low, J., & Perner, J. (2012). Implicit and explicit theory of mind:State of the art. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 1-13, doi:10.1111/bjdp.2012.30.issue-1. Low, J., & Watts, J. (2013). Attributing false beliefs about object identity reveals a signature blind spot in humans'efficient mind-reading system. Psychological Science, 24, 305-311, doi:10.1177/0956797612451469. Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255-258, doi:10.1126/science.1107621. Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337-344, doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01362-5. Perner, J., & Roessler, J. (2012). From infants'to children's appreciation of belief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 519-525, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.004. Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers'executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 567-579, doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515-526, doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512. Pyers, J. E., & Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding:Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science, 20, 805-812, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02377.x. Rubio-Fernández, P., & Glucksberg, S. (2012). Reasoning about other people's beliefs:Bilinguals have an advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 211-217, doi:10.1037/a0025162. Ruffman, T., Garnham, W., Import, A., & Connolly, D. (2001). Does eye gaze indicate implicit knowledge of false belief? Charting transitions in knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 201-224, doi:10.1006/jecp.2001.2633. Schneider, D., Bayliss, A. P., Becker, S. I., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Eye movements reveal sustained implicit processing of others'mental states. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 141, 433-438, doi:10.1037/a0025458. Schneider, D., Lam, R., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Cognitive load disrupts implicit theory-of-mind processing. Psychological Science, 23, 842-847, doi:10.1177/0956797612439070. Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition, 129, 410-417, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.004. Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., & Dux, P. E. (2015). What do we know about implicit false-belief tracking? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1-12. Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes:An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883-885, doi:10.1126/science.1176170. Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587-592, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x. Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580-586, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development:The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655-684, doi:10.1111/cdev.2001.72.issue-3. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs:Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128, doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5. Yott, J., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2012). Breaking the rules:Do infants have a true understanding of false belief? 30:156-171. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 156-171, doi:10.1111/bjdp.2012.30.issue-1.
|