心理与行为研究 ›› 2024, Vol. 22 ›› Issue (1): 79-85.DOI: 10.12139/j.1672-0628.2024.01.011
张姝玥1,2, 赵峰1, 黄骏青1, 彭春花1, 王军利1,*()
收稿日期:
2021-12-20
出版日期:
2024-01-20
发布日期:
2024-01-20
通讯作者:
王军利
基金资助:
Shuyue ZHANG1,2, Feng ZHAO1, Junqing HUANG1, Chunhua PENG1, Junli WANG1,*()
Received:
2021-12-20
Online:
2024-01-20
Published:
2024-01-20
Contact:
Junli WANG
摘要:
分享作为幼儿典型的亲社会行为之一,是幼儿利他意图的表现,对幼儿社会适应有积极意义。已有研究表明,消极道德情绪(如,内疚、羞耻等)可以促进幼儿的分享行为,但积极道德情绪(如,自豪、美好等)对幼儿分享行为的促进作用尚有待进一步考察。因此,本研究通过想象和实际两种启动范式,考察积极道德情绪对3~5岁幼儿分享行为的影响。结果表明:(1)启动积极道德情绪的幼儿分享行为显著多于对照组,积极道德情绪对幼儿的分享行为具有促进作用;(2)幼儿的分享行为发展具有年龄特征,5岁幼儿的分享行为较3岁幼儿更为突出。
中图分类号:
张姝玥, 赵峰, 黄骏青, 彭春花, 王军利. 积极道德情绪对3~5岁幼儿分享行为的影响[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(1): 79-85.
Shuyue ZHANG, Feng ZHAO, Junqing HUANG, Chunhua PENG, Junli WANG. The Influence of Positive Moral Emotions on Sharing Behavior in 3- to 5-Year-Old Children[J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2024, 22(1): 79-85.
年龄 | 实验组 | 控制组 | |||||
n | n1 | (n1/n)×100% | n | n0 | (n0/n)×100% | ||
3岁 | 24 | 20 | 83.33% | 24 | 20 | 83.33% | |
4岁 | 28 | 25 | 89.29% | 23 | 20 | 86.96% | |
5岁 | 27 | 26 | 96.30% | 24 | 22 | 91.67% | |
总计 | 79 | 71 | 89.87% | 71 | 62 | 87.32% |
表1 实验1幼儿道德情绪启动成功率
年龄 | 实验组 | 控制组 | |||||
n | n1 | (n1/n)×100% | n | n0 | (n0/n)×100% | ||
3岁 | 24 | 20 | 83.33% | 24 | 20 | 83.33% | |
4岁 | 28 | 25 | 89.29% | 23 | 20 | 86.96% | |
5岁 | 27 | 26 | 96.30% | 24 | 22 | 91.67% | |
总计 | 79 | 71 | 89.87% | 71 | 62 | 87.32% |
年龄 | n | 实验组 | 控制组 | 总计 |
3岁 | 40 | 4.05±1.45 | 2.95±1.88 | 3.50±1.75 |
4岁 | 45 | 4.84±1.27 | 4.30±1.61 | 4.60±1.44 |
5岁 | 48 | 5.19±1.24 | 4.41±1.28 | 4.83±1.31 |
总计 | 133 | 4.75±1.38 | 3.90±1.71 |
表2 实验1幼儿分享行为(M±SD)
年龄 | n | 实验组 | 控制组 | 总计 |
3岁 | 40 | 4.05±1.45 | 2.95±1.88 | 3.50±1.75 |
4岁 | 45 | 4.84±1.27 | 4.30±1.61 | 4.60±1.44 |
5岁 | 48 | 5.19±1.24 | 4.41±1.28 | 4.83±1.31 |
总计 | 133 | 4.75±1.38 | 3.90±1.71 |
年龄 | 实验组 | 控制组 | |||||
n | n1 | (n1/n)×100% | n | n0 | (n0/n)×100% | ||
3岁 | 23 | 20 | 86.96% | 23 | 20 | 86.96% | |
4岁 | 22 | 20 | 90.91% | 22 | 20 | 90.91% | |
5岁 | 23 | 22 | 95.65% | 22 | 20 | 90.91% | |
总计 | 68 | 62 | 91.18% | 67 | 60 | 89.55% |
表3 实验2幼儿道德情绪启动成功率
年龄 | 实验组 | 控制组 | |||||
n | n1 | (n1/n)×100% | n | n0 | (n0/n)×100% | ||
3岁 | 23 | 20 | 86.96% | 23 | 20 | 86.96% | |
4岁 | 22 | 20 | 90.91% | 22 | 20 | 90.91% | |
5岁 | 23 | 22 | 95.65% | 22 | 20 | 90.91% | |
总计 | 68 | 62 | 91.18% | 67 | 60 | 89.55% |
年龄 | n | 实验组 | 控制组 | 总计 |
3岁 | 40 | 4.35±1.23 | 4.05±1.31 | 4.20±1.26 |
4岁 | 40 | 4.85±1.09 | 4.40±1.27 | 4.63±1.19 |
5岁 | 42 | 5.23±0.97 | 4.65±0.99 | 4.95±1.01 |
总计 | 122 | 4.82±1.14 | 4.37±1.21 |
表4 实验2幼儿分享行为(M±SD)
年龄 | n | 实验组 | 控制组 | 总计 |
3岁 | 40 | 4.35±1.23 | 4.05±1.31 | 4.20±1.26 |
4岁 | 40 | 4.85±1.09 | 4.40±1.27 | 4.63±1.19 |
5岁 | 42 | 5.23±0.97 | 4.65±0.99 | 4.95±1.01 |
总计 | 122 | 4.82±1.14 | 4.37±1.21 |
陈英和, 白柳, 李龙凤. 道德情绪的特点、发展及其对行为的影响. 心理与行为研究, 2015, 13 (5): 627- 636.
DOI |
|
黄玺, 梁宏宇, 李放, 陈世民, 王巍欣, 林妙莲, 郑雪. (2018). 道德提升感: 一种提升道德情操的积极道德情绪. 心理科学进展, 26(7), 1253–1263. | |
刘秀丽. 学前儿童心理理论及欺骗发展的关系研究. 心理发展与教育, 2005, 21 (4): 13- 18.
DOI |
|
任海霞, 崔丽莹. (2018). 国外儿童的道德情绪研究与教育启示. 外国中小学教育, (4), 30–35, 10. | |
任俊, 高肖肖. 道德情绪: 道德行为的中介调节. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19 (8): 1224- 1232. | |
汤明, 李伟强, 刘福会, 袁博. 内疚与亲社会行为的关系: 来自元分析的证据. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27 (5): 773- 788. | |
张姝玥, 赵峰, 彭春花, 王军利, 徐科朋. 积极道德情绪和年龄对3~5岁幼儿安慰行为的影响. 心理科学, 2021, 44 (3): 575- 582.
DOI |
|
赵章留, 寇彧. 儿童四种典型亲社会行为发展的特点. 心理发展与教育, 2006, 22 (1): 117- 121.
DOI |
|
Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J.. Witnessing excellence in action: The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2009, 4 (2): 105- 127.
DOI |
|
Beier, J. S., Gross, J. T., Brett, B. E., Stern, J. A., Martin, D. R., & Cassidy, J.. Helping, sharing, and comforting in young children: Links to individual differences in attachment. Child Development, 2019, 90 (2): e273- e289.
DOI |
|
Berndsen, M., & Feather, N. T.. Reflecting on schadenfreude: Serious consequences of a misfortune for which one is not responsible diminish previously expressed schadenfreude; the role of immorality appraisals and moral emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 2016, 40 (6): 895- 913.
DOI |
|
Brownell, C. A., Iesue, S. S., Nichols, S. R., & Svetlova, M.. Mine or yours? Development of sharing in toddlers in relation to ownership understanding. Child Development, 2013, 84 (3): 906- 920.
DOI |
|
Davidov, M., Vaish, A., Knafo-Noam, A., & Hastings, P. D.. The motivational foundations of prosocial behavior from a developmental perspective-evolutionary roots and key psychological mechanisms: Introduction to the special section. Child Development, 2016, 87 (6): 1655- 1667.
DOI |
|
Drummond, J. D. K., Hammond, S. I., Satlof-Bedrick, E., Waugh, W. E., & Brownell, C. A.. Helping the one you hurt: Toddlers’ rudimentary guilt, shame, and prosocial behavior after harming another. Child Development, 2017, 88 (4): 1382- 1397.
DOI |
|
Eisenberg, N.. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 2000, 51, 665- 697.
DOI |
|
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Sadovsky, A. (2006). Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 517–549). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. | |
Fredrickson, B. L.. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 2001, 56 (3): 218- 226.
DOI |
|
Gummerum, M., Hanoch, Y., & Keller, M.. When child development meets economic game theory: An interdisciplinary approach to investigating social development. Human Development, 2008, 51 (4): 235- 261.
DOI |
|
Gummerum, M., Hanoch, Y., Keller, M., Parsons, K., & Hummel, A.. Preschoolers’ allocations in the dictator game: The role of moral emotions. Journal of Economic Psychology, 2010, 31 (1): 25- 34.
DOI |
|
Haidt, J. (2003). Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 275–289). Washington DC: American Psychological Association. | |
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M.. Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature, 2011, 476 (7360): 328- 331.
DOI |
|
Hart, D., & Matsuba, M. K. (2007). The development of pride and moral life. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 114–133). New York: The Guilford Press. | |
Johnston, M., & Krettenauer, T.. Moral self and moral emotion expectancies as predictors of anti- and pro-social behaviour in adolescence: A case for mediation. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2011, 8 (2): 228- 243.
DOI |
|
Knafo, A., & Plomin, R.. Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: Genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Developmental Psychology, 2006, 42 (5): 771- 786.
DOI |
|
Krettenauer, T., & Johnston, M.. Positively versus negatively charged moral emotion expectancies in adolescence: The role of situational context and the developing moral self. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2011, 29 (3): 475- 488.
DOI |
|
Krettenauer, T., Malti, T., & Sokol, B. W.. The development of moral emotion expectancies and the happy victimizer phenomenon: A critical review of theory and application. International Journal of Developmental Science, 2008, 2 (3): 221- 235.
DOI |
|
Malti, T., Peplak, J., & Acland, E. (2020). Emotional experiences in moral contexts: Developmental perspectives. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of moral development: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 244–263). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | |
Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Developmental transformations in appraisals for pride, shame, and guilt. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 64–113). New York: The Guilford Press. | |
Ongley, S. F., & Malti, T.. The role of moral emotions in the development of children’s sharing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 2014, 50 (4): 1148- 1159.
DOI |
|
Sabato, H., & Eyal, T.. Proud to help when I should: Children’s positive emotions following sharing decisions with a needy versus not-needy other. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2022, 219, 105400.
DOI |
|
Shlefer, S., & Kogut, T.. How did it feel? Affect as a feedback system in repeated donation decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2021, 97, 104203.
DOI |
|
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J.. Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 2007, 58, 345- 372.
DOI |
|
Tignor, S. M., & Colvin, C. R.. The interpersonal adaptiveness of dispositional guilt and shame: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Personality, 2017, 85 (3): 341- 363.
DOI |
|
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M.. Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 2009, 45 (2): 534- 543.
DOI |
|
van de Vyver, J., & Abrams, D.. Testing the prosocial effectiveness of the prototypical moral emotions: Elevation increases benevolent behaviors and outrage increases justice behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2015, 58, 23- 33.
DOI |
|
Vonk, J., Jett, S. E., Tomeny, T. S., Mercer, S. H., & Cwikla, J.. Young children’s theory of mind predicts more sharing with friends over time. Child Development, 2020, 91 (1): 63- 77.
DOI |
|
Weltzien, S., Marsh, L. E., & Hood, B.. Thinking of me: Self-focus reduces sharing and helping in seven- to eight-year-olds. PLoS One, 2018, 13 (1): e0189752.
DOI |
|
Yu, J., Zhu, L. Q., & Leslie, A. M.. Children’s sharing behavior in mini-dictator games: The role of in-group favoritism and theory of mind. Child Development, 2016, 87 (6): 1747- 1757.
DOI |
|
Zhang, H. Y., Chen, S. S., Wang, R., Jiang, J., Xu, Y., & Zhao, H. H.. How upward moral comparison influences prosocial behavioral intention: Examining the mediating role of guilt and the moderating role of moral identity. Frontiers in Psychology, 2017, 8, 1554.
DOI |
[1] | 李卉, 喻昊雪, 吴佳琦. 真还是假?虚幻倾向与事件类型对5~6岁幼儿真实性判断的影响[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(5): 614-620. |
[2] | 刘灵, 韩迎春, 贺晓玲. 网络中积极道德情绪与利他行为:道德提升感与特质性促进定向的作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(5): 667-674. |
[3] | 何明影, 张献英, 李玲玉, 杨丽珠. 3~6岁幼儿羞耻发展教师评定量表的编制[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(2): 231-237. |
[4] | 何慧华, 陈迎新, 蒋琴, 姜露. 父亲婚姻满意度对1~3岁婴幼儿社会−情绪能力的影响:父子依恋和父亲参与的链式中介作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2022, 20(6): 790-796. |
[5] | 李泉, 冯廷勇. 执行功能影响情绪胜任力的发展:一项学前儿童的追踪研究[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2022, 20(6): 805-812. |
[6] | 孙云瑞, 韩映虹, 吕勇, 刘芳. 人格对幼儿白谎行为的影响:“冷”“热”执行功能的并行中介作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2022, 20(2): 240-247. |
[7] | 李启明, 王纪婷. 家庭氛围、子女生活满意度与心理健康代际传递的关系:亲子性别和子女年龄的差异[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2022, 20(2): 282-288. |
[8] | 李飞飞, 缴润凯, 刘丹, 尹航. 幼儿教师感恩特质与职业使命感的交叉滞后分析[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2021, 19(6): 824-830. |
[9] | 贺晓玲, 刘奕晴, 那琳娜. 3~5岁幼儿颜色映射道德和权力概念的认知发展[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2021, 19(5): 636-641. |
[10] | 黄明明, 陈丽萍, 郭莉萍, 赵守盈. 工作-家庭冲突与幼儿教师离职意向的关系:职业认同感与职业倦怠的序列中介作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2021, 19(5): 679-686. |
[11] | 龚扬, 姜露, 秦雯雯. 祖辈参与教养与幼儿社会能力的关系:母亲育儿压力和母亲教养效能感的链式中介作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2021, 19(3): 348-353. |
[12] | 李甜甜, 顾吉有, 徐鹤文. 父亲抑郁对幼儿问题行为的影响:教养效能感和教养方式的中介作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2021, 19(1): 66-73. |
[13] | 王嵩, 李甦. 幼儿复述能力的发展及其与言语工作记忆的关系[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2020, 18(5): 596-602. |
[14] | 魏玲, 余宥依, 刘人豪, 程一智. 场景的年龄特征会影响自闭症谱系障碍儿童的注视模式[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2020, 18(5): 679-685. |
[15] | 闻明晶, 滕树元, 冯晓杭, 田金来, 张向葵. 祖辈共同养育模式下幼儿母子依恋对问题行为的影响:祖孙依恋的中介作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2020, 18(2): 208-213. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||