心理与行为研究 ›› 2024, Vol. 22 ›› Issue (4): 442-449.DOI: 10.12139/j.1672-0628.2024.04.002
收稿日期:
2023-09-14
出版日期:
2024-07-20
发布日期:
2024-07-20
通讯作者:
王敬欣
基金资助:
Min CHANG1, Kuo ZHANG2, Yue SUN3, Sha LI4, Jingxin WANG*,3()
Received:
2023-09-14
Online:
2024-07-20
Published:
2024-07-20
Contact:
Jingxin WANG
摘要:
采用眼动追踪技术,探讨汉语阅读中预测加工普遍发生在词汇水平还是语义水平。操纵句子中的目标词为高预测词和三种水平的低预测词,高预测词和低预测词之间的语义相似度逐渐降低(L1>L2>L3)。结果发现,在早期眼动指标上,低预测词和高预测词之间的语义相似度越低,读者对低预测词的加工时间越长,即语义相似度效应显著,体现了渐进性的语义预测特征;此外,语境限制性调节预测加工。研究结果支持“语义预测”观点。
常敏, 张阔, 孙悦, 李莎, 王敬欣. 汉语阅读中的语义相似度效应:语义预测的证据[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(4): 442-449.
Min CHANG, Kuo ZHANG, Yue SUN, Sha LI, Jingxin WANG. Semantic Similarity Effects in Chinese Reading: Evidence for Semantic Prediction[J]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 2024, 22(4): 442-449.
条件 | 句子 |
高预测词H | 救援队当前面临的困难是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
低预测词L1 | 救援队当前面临的问题是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
低预测词L2 | 救援队当前面临的挑战是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
低预测词L3 | 救援队当前面临的任务是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
表1 实验材料举例
条件 | 句子 |
高预测词H | 救援队当前面临的困难是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
低预测词L1 | 救援队当前面临的问题是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
低预测词L2 | 救援队当前面临的挑战是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
低预测词L3 | 救援队当前面临的任务是如何移动受伤的游客。 |
高预测词(H) | 低预测词1(L1) | 低预测词2(L2) | 低预测词3(L3) | |
预测性 | 0.83(0.15) | 0.03(0.04) | 0.02(0.03) | 0.02(0.04) |
句子通顺性 | 5.26(0.50) | 5.26(0.44) | 5.25(0.45) | 5.18(0.40) |
词频 | 3.04(0.67) | 2.61(0.85) | 2.64(0.84) | 2.54(0.77) |
首字字频 | 4.19(0.57) | 4.17(0.61) | 4.13(0.65) | 4.13(0.61) |
尾字字频 | 4.12(0.57) | 4.22(0.64) | 4.03(0.83) | 4.17(0.64) |
整词笔画数 | 16.25(4.44) | 16.83(4.29) | 16.88(4.83) | 15.85(4.85) |
首字笔画数 | 8.00(2.60) | 8.34(2.99) | 8.78(3.12) | 7.71(3.33) |
尾字笔画数 | 8.25(3.02) | 8.49(3.18) | 8.10(3.17) | 8.14(3.40) |
语义相似度 | 1.00(0.00) | 0.69(0.11) | 0.58(0.11) | 0.47(0.11) |
表2 目标词信息的平均值和标准差[M(SD)]
高预测词(H) | 低预测词1(L1) | 低预测词2(L2) | 低预测词3(L3) | |
预测性 | 0.83(0.15) | 0.03(0.04) | 0.02(0.03) | 0.02(0.04) |
句子通顺性 | 5.26(0.50) | 5.26(0.44) | 5.25(0.45) | 5.18(0.40) |
词频 | 3.04(0.67) | 2.61(0.85) | 2.64(0.84) | 2.54(0.77) |
首字字频 | 4.19(0.57) | 4.17(0.61) | 4.13(0.65) | 4.13(0.61) |
尾字字频 | 4.12(0.57) | 4.22(0.64) | 4.03(0.83) | 4.17(0.64) |
整词笔画数 | 16.25(4.44) | 16.83(4.29) | 16.88(4.83) | 15.85(4.85) |
首字笔画数 | 8.00(2.60) | 8.34(2.99) | 8.78(3.12) | 7.71(3.33) |
尾字笔画数 | 8.25(3.02) | 8.49(3.18) | 8.10(3.17) | 8.14(3.40) |
语义相似度 | 1.00(0.00) | 0.69(0.11) | 0.58(0.11) | 0.47(0.11) |
高预测词(H) | 低预测词1(L1) | 低预测词2(L2) | 低预测词3(L3) | |
跳读率(%) | 37(1) | 31(1) | 33(1) | 34(1) |
首次注视时间(ms) | 242(3) | 247(3) | 245(2) | 256(3) |
单一注视时间(ms) | 240(3) | 245(3) | 244(3) | 255(3) |
凝视时间(ms) | 258(4) | 271(4) | 278(4) | 282(4) |
回视路径阅读时间(ms) | 312(8) | 330(7) | 335(8) | 349(9) |
总注视时间(ms) | 322(6) | 345(6) | 355(6) | 365(7) |
表3 目标词眼动指标的平均值和标准误[M(SE)]
高预测词(H) | 低预测词1(L1) | 低预测词2(L2) | 低预测词3(L3) | |
跳读率(%) | 37(1) | 31(1) | 33(1) | 34(1) |
首次注视时间(ms) | 242(3) | 247(3) | 245(2) | 256(3) |
单一注视时间(ms) | 240(3) | 245(3) | 244(3) | 255(3) |
凝视时间(ms) | 258(4) | 271(4) | 278(4) | 282(4) |
回视路径阅读时间(ms) | 312(8) | 330(7) | 335(8) | 349(9) |
总注视时间(ms) | 322(6) | 345(6) | 355(6) | 365(7) |
白学军, 曹玉肖, 顾俊娟, 郭志英, 闫国利. 可预测性和空格对中文阅读影响的眼动研究. 心理科学, 2011, 34 (6): 1282- 1288.
|
|
李琳, 赵赛男, 张俐娟, 王敬欣. 老年人汉语阅读中预测误差成本的产生机制. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30 (1): 1- 14.
|
|
张慢慢, 胡惠兰, 张志超, 李鑫, 汪强, 白学军, 臧传丽. 预测性对快速读者和慢速读者词汇加工的影响. 心理学报, 2023, 55 (1): 79- 93.
|
|
赵赛男, 李琳, 张俐娟, 王敬欣. 汉语阅读中高限制性语境对不可预测词加工的影响: 眼动研究. 心理与行为研究, 2021, 19 (6): 736- 742.
|
|
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 2015, 67 (1): 1- 48.
|
|
Bonhage, C. E., Mueller, J. L., Friederici, A. D., & Fiebach, C. J. Combined eye tracking and fMRI reveals neural basis of linguistic predictions during sentence comprehension. Cortex, 2015, 68, 33- 47.
DOI |
|
Brothers, T., & Kuperberg, G. R. Word predictability effects are linear, not logarithmic: Implications for probabilistic models of sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 2021, 116, 104174.
|
|
Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS One, 2010, 5 (6): e10729.
DOI |
|
Carter, B. T., Foster, B., Muncy, N. M., & Luke, S. G. Linguistic networks associated with lexical, semantic and syntactic predictability in reading: A fixation-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 2019, 189, 224- 240.
DOI |
|
Chang, M., Zhang, K., Sun, Y., Li, S., & Wang, J. X. The graded predictive pre-activation in Chinese sentence reading: Evidence from eye movements. Frontiers in Psychology, 2023, 14, 1136488.
DOI |
|
Cui, L., Zang, C. L., Xu, X. C., Zhang, W. X., Su, Y. H., & Liversedge, S. P. Predictability effects and parafoveal processing of compound words in natural Chinese reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2022, 75 (1): 18- 29.
DOI |
|
Delong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 2005, 8 (8): 1117- 1121.
DOI |
|
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 1999, 41 (4): 469- 495.
DOI |
|
Federmeier, K. D., McLennan, D. B., de Ochoa, E., & Kutas, M. The impact of semantic memory organization and sentence context information on spoken language processing by younger and older adults: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 2002, 39 (2): 133- 146.
DOI |
|
Federmeier, K. D., Wlotko, E. W., De Ochoa-Dewald, E., & Kutas, M. Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain Research, 2007, 1146, 75- 84.
|
|
Frank, S. L., Otten, L. J., Galli, G., & Vigliocco, G. The ERP response to the amount of information conveyed by words in sentences. Brain and Language, 2015, 140, 1- 11.
DOI |
|
Frisson, S., Harvey, D. R., & Staub, A. No prediction error cost in reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 2017, 95, 200- 214.
DOI |
|
Günther, F., Dudschig, C., & Kaup, B. Latent semantic analysis cosines as a cognitive similarity measure: Evidence from priming studies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2016, 69 (4): 626- 653.
|
|
Huettig, F., & Mani, N. (2016). Is prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 19–31.
|
|
Ito, A., Corley, M., Pickering, M. J., Martin, A. E., & Nieuwland, M. S. Predicting form and meaning: Evidence from brain potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 2016, 86, 157- 171.
DOI |
|
Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R. Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2004, 16 (1–2): 262- 284.
DOI |
|
Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59.
|
|
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 1984, 307 (5947): 161- 163.
|
|
Li, X. S., & Pollatsek, A. An integrated model of word processing and eye-movement control during Chinese reading. Psychological Review, 2020, 127 (6): 1139- 1162.
DOI |
|
Liu, Y. P., Guo, S. Y., Yu, L., & Reichle, E. D. Word predictability affects saccade length in Chinese reading: An evaluation of the dynamic-adjustment model. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2018, 25 (5): 1891- 1899.
|
|
Luke, S. G., & Christianson, K. Limits on lexical prediction during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 2016, 88, 22- 60.
|
|
McRae, K., de Sa, V. R., & Seidenberg, M. S. On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1997, 126 (2): 99- 130.
DOI |
|
Pan, J. E., Yan, M., Richter, E. M., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. The Beijing sentence corpus: A Chinese sentence corpus with eye movement data and predictability norms. Behavior Research Methods, 2021, 54 (4): 1989- 2000.
DOI |
|
Pynte, J., New, B., & Kennedy, A. On-line contextual influences during reading normal text: A multiple-regression analysis. Vision Research, 2008, 48 (21): 2172- 2183.
|
|
Rayner, K., Li, X. S., Juhasz, B. J., & Yan, G. L. The effect of word predictability on the eye movements of Chinese readers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2005, 12 (6): 1089- 1093.
|
|
Rayner, K., & Well, A. D. Effects of contextual constraint on eye movements in reading: A further examination. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1996, 3 (4): 504- 509.
|
|
Roland, D., Yun, H., Koenig, J. P., & Mauner, G. Semantic similarity, predictability, and models of sentence processing. Cognition, 2012, 122 (3): 267- 279.
DOI |
|
Schuster, S., Hawelka, S., Richlan, F., Ludersdorfer, P., & Hutzler, F. Eyes on words: A fixation-related fMRI study of the left occipito-temporal cortex during self-paced silent reading of words and pseudowords. Scientific Reports, 2015, 5 (1): 12686.
DOI |
|
Song, Y., Shi, S. M., Li, J., & Zhang, H. S. (2018). Directional skip-gram: Explicitly distinguishing left and right context for word embeddings. In Proceedings of 2018 Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies (pp. 175–180). New Orleans, LA: Association for Computational Linguistics.
|
|
Staub, A. The effect of lexical predictability on eye movements in reading: Critical review and theoretical interpretation. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2015, 9 (8): 311- 327.
DOI |
|
Taylor, W. L. “Cloze Procedure”: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism Quarterly, 1953, 30 (4): 415- 433.
|
|
Traxler, M. J. Trends in syntactic parsing: Anticipation, Bayesian estimation, and good-enough parsing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2014, 18 (11): 605- 611.
DOI |
|
Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern applied statistics with S (4th ed.). New York: Springer.
|
|
Westfall, J. (2016). PANGEA: Power analysis for general ANOVA designs. Retrieved June 1, 2023, from https://github.com/jake-westfall/pangea
|
|
Yun, H., Mauner, G., Roland, D., & Koenig, J. P. (2012). The effect of semantic similarity is a function of contextual constraint. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1191–1196). Austin, TX: The Cognitive Science Society.
|
[1] | 王永胜, 张娜, 杜小普, 郭紫璐, 李馨. 语素意识在汉语二语学习者阅读过程中的作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(4): 450-456. |
[2] | 严晨毓, 徐琴美, 刘涛, 成梦婷, 马洒, 葛雨箐. 婴儿能否感知音乐情绪?来自预期违背范式的证据[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(4): 494-500. |
[3] | 张锦坤, 赖廷明, 昝晓琪, 李莎, 连坤予, 张俐娟. 多媒体学习中的不流畅效应:线索与流畅性的平衡[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(4): 545-552. |
[4] | 张文, 阚超, 郭丽敏, 刘知和, 刘阳. 排球运动员接扣球知觉预测认知加工特征研究:来自眼动和fNIRS的关联证据[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(4): 562-569. |
[5] | 王影超, 李赛男, 宋子明, 闫国利. 不同阅读方式对汉语句子阅读中词频效应的影响[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(2): 183-188, 226. |
[6] | 崔晶晶, 汪洋, 李笑, 杨海波. 问题性社交媒体使用大学生注意抑制受损的特异性[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2024, 22(2): 258-265. |
[7] | 陈汝淇, 包亚倩, 黄林洁琼, 李兴珊. 中文阅读中词语加工与眼动控制整合模型简介[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(6): 725-735. |
[8] | 于秒, 王文娣, 陈晓霄. 汉语“N的V”结构加工的韵律制约[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(6): 744-750. |
[9] | 鹿子佳, 张志超, 符颖, 张慢慢, 臧传丽, 白学军. 重复词无法帮助中文读者获得副中央凹词类信息[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(5): 577-584. |
[10] | 阿依古丽·艾尼, 买合甫来提·坎吉, 刘贵雄, 帕里扎·布拉提汗. 词间空格对维吾尔族大学生词汇加工的影响[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(2): 163-168. |
[11] | 张俐娟, 张凤筠, 赵赛男, 王敬欣. 合理性对汉语阅读中双字词语义预视效益的优势作用:眼动研究[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(1): 12-19. |
[12] | 丁辉, 张志超, 张慢慢, 臧传丽. 语境影响反语理解的眼动研究元分析[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2023, 21(1): 28-35. |
[13] | 金雪莲, 姜英杰. 孤独症儿童自我−他人来源记忆监测损伤:学习时间分配的作用[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2022, 20(6): 768-774. |
[14] | 贾宁, 容丽卓, 代景华. 社会性线索对内隐和外显元认知监控的影响[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2022, 20(5): 593-599. |
[15] | 李士一, 谢岩枫, 赵光, 白学军. 媒体多任务经验对不同注意模式下内隐记忆的影响[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2022, 20(4): 433-440. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||